Included under terms of UK Non-commercial Government License.
NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.
Macdonald G, Livingstone N, Hanratty J, et al. The effectiveness, acceptability and cost-effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for maltreated children and adolescents: an evidence synthesis. Southampton (UK): NIHR Journals Library; 2016 Sep. (Health Technology Assessment, No. 20.69.)
The effectiveness, acceptability and cost-effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for maltreated children and adolescents: an evidence synthesis.
Show detailsIn this chapter we present evidence for the clinical effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of treatment modalities as described in the previous chapters, drawing solely on the evidence of controlled trials or, in the case of economic evidence, decision models. The evidence is organised around intervention groups.
The breadth of this evidence synthesis meant that it was not possible, a priori, to establish a limited number of primary and secondary outcomes. In Chapter 3, we presented descriptively the broad outcome domains that studies reported having measured, whether or not data were presented. In this chapter, and based on what we know about the proximal adverse effects of maltreatment on children’s emotional and psychological well-being, we examine the evidence for the impact of interventions on mental health outcomes, such as post-traumatic effects, depression and anxiety. We then assess the evidence for the effectiveness of interventions on those outcomes that the study authors stated were their intended outcomes; however, we recognise that, in doing so, we may be underestimating biases that are associated with selective outcome reporting, as well as publication bias more generally. Finally, we report any evidence of cost-effectiveness located in the systematic review.
Cognitive–behavioural therapy
We identified 26 controlled studies90–103,106–112,114–117,121,176,268 of cognitive–behavioural interventions (CBT), of which 23 were randomised trials. There were sufficient randomised trials to attempt to explore the differential effect on different maltreatment histories, broadly defined, and so this section is organised into the following three groupings, and does not include the three COSs of CBT interventions.
Description of studies
Of the 11 studies89–103 of CBT interventions for children who have been sexually abused, two89,90 were studies of group-based treatments and nine91–103 were studies of treatments provided to children individually, sometimes in parallel with treatment for the non-offending parent or carer.
Six89–99 of the 11 studies were conducted by a team of clinical researchers who had developed a particular approach to treating children traumatised by sexual abuse, known sometimes as TF-CBT. Although among the most rigorous and well-conducted studies, studies of this particular intervention (and others) are compromised by the lack of independent evaluation.
Location of studies
All studies were conducted in the USA, with the exception of the study by Jaberghaderi et al.,102 which was conducted in Iran, and King et al.,103 which was undertaken in Australia.
Study size
Five studies90,99,101–103 had small samples sizes ranging from 18 to 63 participants. A multisite trial by Cohen 2004,95,96 had a sample of 229 participants. The remainder ranged from 82 to 210 participants.91–94,97,98,100 As a result, the meta-analyses we conducted were not sufficiently powered to detect small, but potentially important, effects. Baseline differences in these studies also proved problematic in drawing any conclusions based on end-point data, as it did for almost all included studies.
Participants
Gender
Three studies90,101,102 focused solely on girls who had been sexually abused. The remaining studies included both boys and girls, with the percentage of boys ranging from 11%89 to 42%.91,92
Age
One study91,92 was concerned with preschoolers (boys and girls aged 3–6 years). Five studies90,95–98 set inclusion criteria for similar age groups: 7–13 years,90,97,98 8– 14 years,95,96 12–13 years102 and 13–18 years.101 Inclusion criteria for the other five studies89,93,94,99,103 ranged from children aged 2–8 years99 to children aged 4–13 years,89 5–17 years103 and 7–15 years.93,94
Maltreatment
The range of abuse experienced by participants was broad, and differently reported, but the following picture of participants emerged. Most were abused by men known to them. The majority of perpetrators were family members. In three studies,89,90,93,94 approximately half of the children and young people had experienced oral, vaginal or anal penetration. In the study91,92 dealing with the youngest participants, the percentage that had experienced vaginal or anal intercourse was 26%.91,92 In the study of children aged 2–8 years,99 the number reported to have experienced penile penetration was 16%. Participants in all studies ranged from those who had experienced one incident of abuse to those who had experienced multiple incidents, sometimes over many years. Many participants also reported the use of force, or threat of force. Not all studies reported detailed abuse data, for example Deblinger 200199 or Jaberghaderi 2004.102 See Table 3 for a profile of participants in each study.
Inclusion criteria
All studies had inclusion criteria that specified contact sexual abuse. All but two trials101,102 made the independent substantiation of sexual abuse an inclusion criterion. Most set cut-off points on the time of last episode of abuse as an inclusion criterion, ranging from 3 months101 through 6 months91–94 to 2 or 3 years.103 Although Deblinger et al.99 did not set a time limit, the authors report that the mean age of the children was 5.45 years (SD 1.47 years) and the mean age of first experience of sexual abuse was 4.5 years (SD 1.47 years), based on mothers’ estimates. The report by Berliner and Saunders89 did not specify inclusion or exclusion criteria, but all participants were said to have provided statements, substantiated by independent assessment, that they had been sexually abused. The Jaberghaderi et al. study102 required that girls had experienced sexual abuse at ≥ 6 months prior to the study.
The presence and severity of symptoms as inclusion criteria were highly variable. Six studies91,92,95–98,101–103 reported the presence of particular symptomatology thresholds as an inclusion criterion. Cohen et al.91,92 required a minimal level of symptomatology defined as a Weekly Behavior Report total behaviour score of > 7 or any sexually inappropriate behaviour reported on the Child Sexual Behavior Inventory (CSBI).259 Cohen et al.95,96 stipulated that participants had to meet five criteria for sexual abuse-related Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition (DSM-IV)-defined PTSD, including at least one in each of the three clusters (re-experiencing, avoidance or numbing and hyperarousal). Deblinger et al.97,98 required the presence of three PTSD symptoms, including at least one symptom of avoidance or re-experiencing the phenomenon. Investigators decided to take both children who met full Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Third Edition, Revised criteria for PTSD and those with partial PTSD symptoms because of the possibility of delayed onset of episodic course. King et al.103 required that the children met diagnostic criteria for PTSD or provided evidence of high risk of developing the disorder. Foa et al.101 required a primary diagnosis (DSM-IV, Text Revision) of chronic or subthreshold PSTD. Jaberghaderi et al.102 recruited girls whose scores on the Child Report of Post-traumatic Symptoms (CROPS)229 indicated a clinically significant level of post-traumatic symptoms.
Interventions and comparison
Two studies99,268 provided a group-based intervention, and the remainder provided individual therapy (IT).
Group treatments
Interventions
In Berliner and Saunders 199689 both groups received a therapy described as a ‘structured equivalent of sexual abuse-specific group therapy’. In the experimental arm, a focus was added on explaining fear (in the session on feelings), stress inoculation therapy (SIT) was substituted for one of the two ‘family and friends’ sessions, two sessions were devoted to GE and SIT principles were applied to sessions on disclosure impact and self-esteem. Children were taught about the automatic nature of fear as a response to danger and how to manage this through progressive relaxation and coping strategies (quieting reflex and thought-stopping). Children were encouraged to practice these skills between group sessions (pp. 299–30).
Deblinger et al. 200199 provided group-based CBT to children and their mothers in separate groups. The parents’ groups covered a range of topics that varied somewhat according to the specific needs of each group but commonly followed the following order and number of sessions: education/coping (three sessions), communication, modelling, GE (two sessions) and behaviour management (six sessions). The children’s group took the form of an interactional behavioural therapy, facilitated by an interactive workbook,507 and which incorporated a range of cognitive–behavioural methods, including GE, modelling, education, coping and body safety training. In addition, members of the CBT group met for an additional 15 minutes each week for a joint parent and child activity session.
Comparisons
The comparison groups in Berliner and Saunders 199689 received conventional sexual abuse-specific group therapy with or without SIT and the specific CBT focus on fear and anxiety. The sessions covered: getting acquainted and establishing ground rules; feelings; family and friends (two sessions); disclosure impact, self-esteem and sexual abuse; body awareness and sexuality (two sessions) and prevention and termination.
Deblinger et al. 200199 compared the effectiveness of group CBT for parents and children with supportive group therapy (for parents) paired with a more didactic, information-based approach for children.
Individual treatments
Those studies headed by Cohen and Deblinger95,96 are essentially evaluations of a manualised programme first developed by the authors in the early nineties.283
Cohen 199591,92 evaluated a manualised, short-term treatment model designed for sexually abused children and their parents, named Cognitive–Behavioural Therapy-Sexually Abused Preschool Children. Children receive safety education and assertiveness training, are helped to identify appropriate compared with inappropriate touching, and to deal with attributions regarding the abuse, ambivalent feelings towards the perpetrator, regressive and inappropriate behaviours, and fear and anxiety. Specific issues for parents include ambivalence in their belief in the child’s account, ambivalent feelings towards the perpetrator, attributions regarding the abuse, concerns that the child is ‘damaged’, how to provide appropriate emotional support for the child and manage inappropriate child behaviours, fear and anxiety. Interventions include the use of cognitive reframing, thought-stopping, positive imagery, contingency reinforcement programmes, parent management training and problem-solving. Psychoeducation and support are embedded in the programme.
Cohen 199893,94 evaluated a programme entitled Sexual Abuse-Specific Cognitive–Behavioural Therapy (SAS-CBT). The programme is not described in detail, but it was designed specifically to address depression, anxiety and behavioural difficulties. SAS-CBT addressed feelings of helplessness (including not being believed), distorted attributions (self-blame) about the abuse and other negative events, feeling damaged/different, and consequent low self-esteem. It incorporated anxiety reduction techniques, such as thought replacement, positive imagery/relaxation, enhancement of safety and management of intrusive thoughts. It helped children to address behavioural problems by teaching them about the connections between thoughts, feelings and behaviour, management techniques and problem-solving skills. The focus in the parents’ groups was on reducing their emotional distress (again, including addressing distorted attributions, anxiety and anger), enhancing their ability to support their child and behaviour management.
Deblinger 199697,98 explored variations of a programme described as similar to that of Cohen and Mannarino.91,92 Participants were assigned to one of three experimental conditions: child only, parent only or combined child and parent. Children in all of the experimental arms received an intervention that included GE, modelling, education, coping and body safety skills. GE was described as the cornerstone of the intervention aimed at helping the children to disconnect the associations frequently made between highly negative emotions and abuse-related thoughts, discussion and other reminders. Parents in the experimental arms were taught how to respond therapeutically to their children’s behaviours and needs, that is, how to reduce their fears and avoidance behaviours (through the use of modelling, GE and processing exercises); how to analyse their own interactions with their children behaviourally, thus identifying those situations when they might inadvertently have reinforced problem behaviours and the maintenance of PTSD symptoms; and child management skills.
Cohen 200495,96 delivered the same manualised TF-CBT intervention used in earlier studies by this team, but in a more representative sample of children across two sites. This manualised intervention also forms the basis of the study conducted by Deblinger et al.100 in 2011. In this paper100 the authors describe the TF-CBT intervention evaluated in this study as including components ‘that spell out the acronym PRACTICE:
- Psychoeducation and parenting
- Relaxation
- Affective modulation
- Cognitive coping
- Trauma narrative (TN)
- In vivo exposure
- Conjoint parent–child sessions, and
- Enhancing safety and future development’ (p. 69; © 2010 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Reproduced with permission).
In this study,100 the authors were concerned to investigate the importance of the TN to effective treatment of children with PTSD. This four-arm trial compared two versions of TF-CBT (as described for Deblinger 200199), one with, and one without, the inclusion of the TN component and at the same time manipulated the length of treatment and degree of time given to the TN. The authors report that in all conditions both children and parents received psychoeducation about CSA and skill-building (e.g. relaxation, affective modulation, cognitive coping and body safety training), as well as parenting skills training. However, only those children assigned to the two TN groups ‘were actively encouraged to develop a detailed narrative about the sexual abuse and related experiences, which they processed and reviewed with the therapist as well as their non-offending parent’ (p. 69).100 Children in the eight-session TN condition spent three to four sessions on the TN component; this was at least doubled in the 16-session condition.
King et al.103 evaluated the effectiveness of two CBT interventions, both of which the authors say were particularly influenced by the work of Deblinger et al.208,485 The first was a child-only intervention. This began with a session that specified the problem areas, presented the rationale for the programme and set goals. The following three sessions focused on teaching coping skills to enable children to deal with disturbing memories of abuse and their feelings of anxiety and guilt (relaxation training, behaviour rehearsal and cognitive therapy). Sessions 5 through to 18 focused on graded exposure, and sessions 19–20 on relapse prevention and education, including personal safety skills. The second intervention was family CBT, in which the child received the programme outlined above, and non-offending mothers also received a CBT intervention. The parent intervention began with the rationale of the programme and issues relating to CSA, followed by nine sessions ‘on the development of parent–child communication skills in order to facilitate listening and problem sharing and to overcome avoidance of abuse-related discussion within the family’ (p. 1350).103 The remaining 10 sessions focused on child behaviour management, including antecedent stimulus control and contingency management. Parents were encouraged to monitor their own emotional responses in order to provide an appropriate coping model for the child.
Celano et al.90 evaluated the impact of the Recovering from Abuse Program, an eight-session group that focused on children’s maladaptive beliefs, affects and behaviour along four dimensions: self-blame/stigmatisation; betrayal; traumatic sexualisation and powerlessness.
Jaberghaderi 2004102 compared individual CBT with EMDR, and Foa 2013101 compared exposure therapy with supportive counselling.
Foa 2013101 evaluated the effectiveness of prolonged exposure therapy, delivered in eight modules, comprising (1) explaining the treatment rationale; (2) establishing an index of trauma and teaching participants breathing control; (3) presenting common reactions to trauma; (4) explaining the rationale for in vivo exposure, establishing an in vivo hierarchy and arranging homework for the participant; (5) two to five sessions of imaginal exposure lasting between 15 and 45 minutes, combined with reprocessing of the experiences; (6) four to seven further sessions of imaginal exposure centred on the most extreme periods of trauma; (7) generalisation of newly acquired skills and relapse prevention; and (8) a final project, ‘such as making booklets about the trauma and the gains made in treatment’ (Foa 2013101 p. 2652).
Comparisons
Four studies91–94,101 compared CBT for children and parents with non-directive supportive therapies:
Cohen 200495,96 compared TF-CBT with child-centred therapy (CCT), described as ‘child/parent[-]centred treatment model focused on establishing a trusting therapeutic relationship that is self-affirming, empowering, and validating for the parent and child . . . Therapists provided active listening, reflection, accurate empathy, encouragement to talk about feelings, and belief in the child’s and parent’s ability to develop positive coping strategies for abuse-related difficulties . . . Although sessions were generally client directed, written psychoeducational information about CSA was provided, and children, specifically, were prompted to share their feelings about sexual abuse during two therapy sessions if they did not do so spontaneously’95 (p. 398; reproduced with permission).
Deblinger 199697,98 and King et al.103 included a community control and wait-list group, respectively.
Celano et al.90 compared the efficacy of CBT provided to children and their mothers with supportive, unstructured psychotherapy, also to children and their mothers.
Jaberghaderi 2004102 compared individual CBT with EMDR.
Deblinger 2011100 explored the differential effectiveness of eight sessions compared with 16 sessions of TF-CBT, with or without a TN component, that is, a four-arm trial.
Number and duration of sessions
Group treatments
The group-based therapies were provided over 10 and 11 sessions, respectively.89,99 For the experimental group, Berliner 199689 augmented the conventional sexual abuse specific group therapy provided to the control group with sessions specifically explaining the nature of fear, the principles of SIT and their application to disclosure impact and self-esteem. In the Deblinger 200199 study, parents and children met for a joint group session of 15 minutes each week.
Individual treatments
The individual therapies were provided for between 8 and 20 sessions. Participants in the Celano et al.90 study had eight sessions of 1 hour each. In all but two or three sessions, the therapist spent half of the time with the mother and half with the child. The remaining sessions were conducted conjointly.
Three studies91–93,95,96 provided around 1.5 hours per week, divided between parents and child over 8 and 12 weeks, respectively.
Deblinger 199697,98 provided therapy in 12 × 45-minute sessions to participants in the parent-only and child-only arms. In the parent-and-child arm, therapy also entailed 12 sessions, but this time of up to 90 minutes. In King 2006,103 all participants each received 20 × 50-minute sessions. This means that, in the parent-and-child arm, 40 × 50-minute sessions were provided.
Adolescent girls in Foa 2013101 received up to 14 sessions of between 60 and 90 minutes, and in the Jaberghaderi et al.102 study they received up to 12 sessions of 45 minutes in the experimental group and 30 in the EMDR comparison group.
Participants in Deblinger 2011100 were allocated to one of four TF-CBT treatment conditions: eight sessions with a TN component; eight sessions without a TN component; 16 sessions with a TN component; 16 sessions without a TN component. Sessions were each 90 minutes, usually divided into two 45-minute individual sessions for the child and caregiver, respectively. Some sessions included 30 minutes of conjoint parent–child time.
Outcomes and measures used in studies of cognitive–behavioural therapy for sexually abused children
Outcomes assessed using the same measure
When the impact of intervention on parenting practices was assessed, the measure used was the Parenting Practices Questionnaire (PPQ).274
In each study that examined child depression, child sexual behaviour and child behaviour, the same measures were used, namely the CDI,80 the CSBI259 and the CBCL,260 respectively, although different authors may cite different sources for the same measure.
Outcomes assessed using different measures
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Six studies90,93–99,103 assessed the impact of CBT on symptoms of post-traumatic stress using six different measures.
In addition, Celano 199690 and King 2000103 used both the PTSD subscale of the CBCL,260 completed by the child’s parent, and one other measure of PTSD.
Celano 199690 used a child report measure [Children’s Impact of Traumatic Events Scale-Revised (CITES-R); Wolfe and Gentile, Department of Psychology, London Health Sciences Centre, London, 1991, unpublished] and King et al.103 used a measure administered by a research assistant (the child version of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV290).
Two studies97–99 used the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version administered to parents;275 two studies95,96,100 used the Kiddie Schedule for Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders, Present and Lifetime Version (KSADS-PL).268
Cohen 199693,94 used the Trauma Symptom Checklist for Children (TSCC325). Jaberghaderi 2004102 used two measures of post-traumatic symptomatology, the Parent Report of Post-traumatic Symptoms (PROPS) and CROPS.288
Foa 2013101 assessed PTSD with the Child PTSD Symptom Scale-Interview286,287 and (as a secondary outcome measure) a self-report version of the same measure.
Anxiety
Of the studies using the Stait-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-C), we used data on STAI-C State subscale (as opposed to STAI-C Trait subscale), as this measures present state anxiety.93–97 Other measures used to assess the impact of interventions on anxiety, included the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS)681 and the STAI-C.682
Fear
Three measures of fear were used in four of the included studies. Berliner and Saunders89 used the Fear Survey Schedule for Children-Revised255 and the Sexual Abuse Fear Evaluation Scales (SAFE).306 King et al.103 used the Fear Thermometer for Sexually Abused Children.291
Study authors typically reported data for study completers rather than for those recruited to the study, for example Cohen and Mannarino.91,92
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of cognitive–behavioural therapy for sexually abused children
Quality aspects were generally not well reported. Of the CBT studies, this is probably the strongest group in terms of risk of bias, although the amount of missing information makes this judgement somewhat speculative.
The difficulties of blinding participants and personnel in studies of psychosocial interventions means that this risk-of-bias domain has largely been assessed as high risk of bias, and, unless there are reasons to believe that the lack of blinding has not resulted in a high risk of bias, we do not comment on this in the following text.
The reliance in many studies on self-report measures, in the absence of other ‘masked’ data collection, also contributes to judgements of high risk of bias in relation to detection bias. On the other hand, we recognise that self-report measures may be a more valid approach to the assessment of some outcomes.
Full details of our assessments of these studies (and all others in this chapter) can be found in Appendix 10. Figure 24 provides an overview of the risk of bias in the body of evidence as a whole for CBT interventions for children who have experienced sexual abuse.
Sequence generation
Sequence allocation was deemed sufficiently robust to be judged low risk of bias in four studies.89,91–94,99 The Jabergadheri et al.102 study was judged as being of ‘unclear’ risk of bias because the authors write ‘Participants were randomly assigned to treatment condition, with some adjustments to promote equivalence between groups’ but then go on to describe a blocked randomisation approach (p. 361). The remaining studies were also judged ‘unclear’, as the authors simply report that participants were randomly assigned.90,95–98,100–103
Allocation concealment
Only two studies89,101 described steps taken to conceal allocation or stated clearly that allocation had been concealed. The Berliner and Saunders89 study reported that ‘Assigned therapists and other staff were blind to the random assignment schedule’ (p. 299) and the Foa et al.101 study reported that ‘On completing the preparatory phase but prior to the patient beginning treatment, a research assistant consulted the randomisation table and notified the therapist of the patient’s treatment condition’ (p. 2651).
No information was provided in the remaining studies. The Jabergadheri et al.102 study was assessed as ‘unclear’ on this basis.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Four trials90,93,94,101,102 were judged as ‘low risk of bias’ for outcome assessment. Foa et al.101 state that ‘assessment was conducted by two psychologists, blind to assignment’ (p. 2651), and Cohen 199893,94 say that ‘The evaluator conducting the initial and follow-up assessments was blind to treatment condition or assignment’ (p. 139); Jaberghaderi et al.102 says that assessment was conducted ‘by two psychologists, blind to assignment’ (p. 362). Celano et al.90 reported that ‘standardised measures were administered by a clinician not involved in the child’s treatment. Additionally, a psychiatrist or psychologist blind to treatment condition rated the child’s overall psychosocial functioning based on child and caretaker interviews’.
The remaining studies91,92,95,96,99,100,103 were deemed as ‘high risk of bias’, as they relied wholly on self-report or parent-report measures.
Incomplete outcome data
Four studies93–96,100,101 were assessed as ‘low risk of bias’ for incomplete outcome data. All four analysed data on an intention-to-treat (ITT) basis, although Cohen 200495,96 used SAS multiple imputation for missing data [release 8.2 (2001); SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA].
Berliner and Saunders89 experienced high levels of attrition and reported on around only 50% of those children who completed at least eight sessions and provided data at one of the follow-up assessment points. The statistical checks undertaken by the authors do not attenuate the likelihood of bias. The levels and approach to attrition was also an issue for Cohen 1996,91,92 King et al.103 and Jabergadheri et al.102 The impact of missing data was less clear in the studies by Celano et al.,90 Deblinger et al.97,98 and Deblinger et al.,99 and these were judged to be ‘unclear’.
Selective outcome reporting
Both Foa et al.101 and Cohen 200495 registered their trials. Foa et al.101 reported findings for the primary outcome listed; there were no secondary outcomes identified on the trial registration (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT02148484). In addition to all outcome measures listed in the trial registration, Cohen et al.95 also report post hoc on changes in child behaviour, but overall we judged this trial to be ‘low risk of bias’ on this domain (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT00000383).
In the absence of study protocols it is extremely difficult to assess the risk of selective outcome reporting. In general, most other studies appeared to indicate that they were reporting on all predetermined outcomes. In that respect, almost all were assessed as ‘low risk of bias’. However, Cohen 199893,94 did not report the results for one measure in their report of initial outcomes, but did report outcomes for all time points at 1-year follow-up. Deblinger 2011100 report only the results of analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for complete sets of data. These two studies93,94,100 were therefore judged to be of ‘high risk of bias’.
Other sources of bias
No other potential sources of bias were identified in these studies. In Jaberghaderi et al.102 the therapists delivering the CBT and EMDR were both authors on the paper, but, as each was delivering the therapy in both arms of the trial, we judged this to be of ‘low risk of bias’.
Results: cognitive–behavioural therapy for sexually abused children
We were able to combine data from different studies for eight outcomes, at up to three time points: immediately post intervention, 3–6 months later and at least 1 year later. When studies had more than one treatment group, these were combined for the purposes of meta-analysis.
The results presented here involve only participants included by the study authors in their analyses, many of which excluded participants due to dropout, uncollected data or for reasons they do not report. Each result reported is the post-test score in the intervention group compared with the control group.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Six studies90,93–99,103 examined the impact of CBT on post-traumatic stress using a variety of scales (see Table 5). These studies90,93–99,103 yielded an average reduction of 0.44 SDs based on various child PTSD scales (95% CI 4.43 to 1.53; I2 = 46%; p-value for heterogeneity 0.10; τ2 = 0.05) immediately after treatment, and three of these93–98 suggested a reduction of 0.38 SDs (95% CI 0.65 to 0.11; I2 = 4%; p-value for heterogeneity 0.35; τ2 = 0.00) after at least 1 year (Figure 2).
In our sensitivity analysis we found that over the range of correlations assumed (ρ= 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1), results for PTSD are robust to whether follow-up measures or change score measures are used. This was the case for both post-test outcomes and 1-year outcomes.
Depression
Five studies90,93–98,103 looked at the impact of CBT on depression in children using the CDI. When combined in a meta-analysis, these five studies yielded an average reduction of 2.83 points on the CDI immediately after intervention (95% CI –4.53 to 1.13; I2 = 22%; p-value for heterogeneity 0.27; τ2 = 0.84) (Figure 3).
Four of these studies89,93–98 sustained an average decrease of 1.42 points (95% CI –2.91 to 0.06; I2 = 0%; p = 0.06; τ2 = 0.0) after at least 1 year (see Figure 3). A decrease of 2.9 on the CDI represents something in excess of a modest change in a scale that previous studies683,684 indicate has a SD of approximately 7. This represents a small to moderate effect size, broadly equivalent to that seen on the anxiety scales in meta-analyses in this review.
In our sensitivity analysis, we found that for both post-test measures and 1-year follow-up measures, the results were closer to ‘no effect’ when using change scores rather than follow-up scores. CIs became wider as the assumed correlation reduced, but even when the correlation is assumed to be 1, the estimated mean difference (MD) is –1.98 for post-test scores with a 95% CI of –4.59 to 0.63, and –0.51 for 1-year follow-up scores with a 95% CI of –3.0 to 2.0.
Anxiety
Five studies89,93–98,103 examined the impact of CBT on anxiety. These yielded an average decrease of 0.23 SDs on various child anxiety scales (95% CI 0.03 to 0.42; I2 = 0%; p-value for heterogeneity 0.84; τ2 = 0.0) immediately after treatment, and four of these89,93–98 reported a sustained decrease of 0.28 SDs (95% CI –0.52 to –0.04; I2 = 0%; p-value for heterogeneity = 0.62; τ2 = 0.0) after at least 1 year (Figure 4).
In our sensitivity analysis, we found that for post-test measures, the results were closer to ‘no effect’ when using change scores rather than follow-up scores. CIs increased as the assumed correlation reduced, but, even when the correlation is assumed to be 1, the estimated SMD is –0.19 with a 95% CI of –0.51 to 0.12. For 1-year follow-up measures, results were robust with similar results using change scores as with follow-up scores, showing no evidence of effect.
Sexualised behaviour
Five studies89,91–96,99 provided conflicting evidence on the effectiveness of CBT in the domain of child behaviour problems, assessed using the CSBI (I2 = 67%, p-value for heterogeneity 0.02; τ2 = 6.81). Two studies89,99 observed increases of 4.7 and 1.7 points and three studies91–96 observed decreases, one91,92 of which was statistically significant. In a meta-analysis, there was no evidence of an effect on average (mean decrease of –0.65 points, 95% CI –3.53 to 2.24 points).
Four studies89,91–96 provided longer-term data. The first of these observed a much smaller increase than the same study in the short term. Overall, the average effect found at the 3- to 6-month assessment point was similar to that immediately after treatment but was not statistically significant (–0.46 points, 95% CI –5.68 to 4.76 points; Figure 5). Note that Cohen 200495,96 did not report data for this outcome because it was not statistically significant. Although we have not been able to retrieve these data, their inclusion is highly unlikely to change the overall conclusion for this outcome.
Externalising behaviour (e.g. aggression, ‘acting out’)
Seven studies89–98,103 provided data on the CBCL, using the externalising behaviour scale. A meta-analysis of standardised differences in means (owing different scoring systems being used for the scale) did not provide evidence of a beneficial effect on average (decrease of 0.12 SDs, 95% CI –0.40 to 0.17). However, the results were inconsistent (I2 = 58%; p-value for heterogeneity 0.03; τ2 = 0.08), with one study89 observing a statistically significant increase and one study observing a statistically significant decrease.97,98
Only five studies90–98 provided longer-term data from which no clear picture merged of either benefit or harm (Figure 6).
Behaviour management skills of parents
Data from three studies95–99 provide information on the effects of involving parents in CBT interventions with their children on their ability to manage their children’s behaviour. All three studies used the PPQ to assess change. Only two95,96,99 of these studies reported follow-up data for 1 year post treatment. A meta-analysis of outcome data from these two studies indicates a decrease in mean scores of –0.89 1 year after treatment (95% CI –4.89 to 3.11). The long-term effects were not statistically significant but raise doubt about the maintenance of change shown in the post-treatment results, which favoured CBT (Figure 7).
Two studies90,95,96 used, respectively, the Parental Reaction to Incest Disclosure Scale and the Parental Support Questionnaire to measure parental belief of their children and support for them. A meta-analysis of standardised differences in means gave a statistically significant increase of 0.3 SDs in favour of CBT (95% CI 0.03 to 0.57) (Figure 8).
Only one study90 examined parental attributions. In this study,90 the author reported small, statistically non-significant improvements on four aspects of parental attributions, using the Parental Attribution Scale. Parents who had been involved in the CBT arm of this study90 were less likely to blame themselves or their child for what had happened, were slightly more optimistic about their child’s future than those in the TAU group and more likely to hold the perpetrator responsible. However, the CIs were very wide, crossing the line of no effect.
The Parent Emotional Reaction Questionnaire (PERQ) is designed to assess stressful parental emotional reactions to the sexual abuse of their children. Parents are asked to endorse the frequency of specific reactions including fear, sadness, guilt, anger, embarrassment, shame and emotional preoccupation. No psychometric data are currently available for this measure. In the two studies95,96,99 that used the PERQ we found a decrease of seven points in parents’ negative reactions (95% CI 3.8 to 10.1). Cohen 200495,96 measured outcomes longer term, and observed a smaller but still statistically significant, decrease of 4.6 points at 1 year.
Additional results: studies of cognitive–behavioural therapy for sexually abused children
The above meta-analyses incorporate most of the data available from the randomised trials of CBT for children who have been sexually abused. Some studies reported on outcomes that are not covered above, or presented data in ways that could not be incorporated. These are briefly summarised here.
King et al.103 assessed the effectiveness of CBT for improving children’s self-efficacy as measured by the (self-report) Coping Questionnaire for Sexually Abused Children (developed by the author) but reported no significant differences between the group receiving CBT and a wait-list control group.
Deblinger 200199 reported a significant difference in favour of CBT for the total score on the CBCL [repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), time/time × group)]. They commented that children in neither group were encouraged to talk in detail about their abusive experiences, owing to their young age, which might account for the smaller reductions in PTSD among the CBT group compared with the control group.
Jaberghaderi et al.102 compared the effectiveness of CBT with EMDR, using two outcome measures. The first was a broad-spectrum, self-report questionnaire-based measure of post-traumatic symptoms (rather than PTSD), CROPS and PROPS.288 The second was a teacher report scale of potential mental disturbance: the Rutter Teacher Scale.289 For post-traumatic symptoms the authors reported large effect sizes (pre- to post-treatment) for both CBT and EMDR and a moderate effect size for the behavioural measure (the Rutter Scale). No significant differences were found between the two treatments. This was a small study102 (n = 14) with no follow-up and was one of the few studies conducted outside the USA.
Foa et al.101 assessed the impact of prolonged exposure therapy using piecewise linear mixed models (LMMs) for continuous data and generalised LMMs for dichotomous data. The authors reported that those treated with prolonged exposure derived greater benefit than those who received supportive counselling, even when delivered by counsellors who typically delivered that form of therapy. Those who received prolonged exposure demonstrated greater improvements on the PTSD symptom severity scale (primary outcome) and on all secondary outcomes, namely self-reported PTSD severity, depression and global functioning. Treatment differences were maintained at 1-year follow-up.
Effectiveness of cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have been sexually abused
Summary
We identified 11 studies of CBT interventions for children and young people who have been sexually abused. Six studies90–96,99,101 compared CBT with supportive, non-directive therapy and two studies97,98,103 compared CBT with no-treatment controls (community and wait-list). One study102 compared CBT with another treatment (EMDR) and two studies89,90 compared variations of CBT treatments, that is CBT with and without a focus on SIT and a focus on fear and anxiety,89 or different exposures of CBT with or without a TN component.100
For children who have been sexually abused, various adaptations of CBT, some offered individually, some in groups and some including work with parents proved of some benefit in reductions in PTSD, depression and anxiety, which were sustained at 1 year post treatment. One102 small study reported equal benefit for CBT and EMDR in reducing PTSD symptoms. There was no evidence of benefit of CBT in reducing sexualised and externalising behaviours. Regarding changes in parents, there was evidence of some improvement in parents’ management of children’s behaviour and support for children, and some change in parents’ attributions regarding the abuse. No harms were reported in any study, but no study set out specifically to examine harms.
This evidence is in line with the conclusions of earlier reviews (e.g. Macdonald et al.,685 Harvey and Taylor686 and de Medeiros Passarela et al.687), suggesting that these approaches may be beneficial compared with non-directive, supportive therapies, but the evidence base remains limited.
Completeness and applicability
None of these studies was conducted in the UK, with most undertaken within the USA. However, the profile of participants is clinically comparable to the population of children who might benefit from child and mental health services in the UK, and the therapies evaluated are recognised, and available, in the UK.
Quality of the evidence
Studies of CBT for children who have been sexually abused could be improved, and would benefit from careful and explicit reporting against Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines. Of some concern is that the field is somewhat dominated by a small team of US researchers who evaluate a version of CBT that they themselves developed.
Economic evidence
One612 economic evaluation, carried out in Australia, explored the cost-effectiveness of CBT for children who have been sexually abused. The study612 used a decision-analytic design to establish the cost–utility of three different treatment strategies for PTSD secondary to childhood sexual abuse, compared with a no-treatment comparator: individual TF-CBT, combined individual TF-CBT plus pharmacotherapy [selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI)], and non-directive counselling. Costs and outcomes were modelled for a hypothetical cohort of 10-year-old children diagnosed with PTSD or PTSD plus depression, subsequent to sexual abuse.
The decision model included a decision tree that modelled the costs and benefits of each treatment during the post-treatment and 12-month follow-up period observed in clinical trials, and a subsequent Markov model that estimated the long-term costs and consequences of the alternative treatments over a 30-year period. The economic evaluation was conducted from the perspective of the Australian mental health care system, and costs and benefits were expressed in 2010–11 Australian dollars (A$) and discounted at a rate of 5% per year.
The model was populated with data obtained from a number of clinical trials and the 2007 Australian Mental Health Survey.402 Resources included in the model were the cost of therapists’ time and the costs of SSRI medication for the combined treatment group. The impact of intervention on the use of other health and social care services (‘knock-on’ effects) was not included. Resources were valued using national published sources for unit costs. Outcomes were reported in terms of QALYs calculated from the Analysis of Quality of Life (AQoL-4D), a generic preference-based instrument included in the 2007 Australian Mental Health Survey. Data from the survey were selected for children and adolescents with a history of childhood sexual abuse, who also met the criteria for PTSD, depression or PTSD and depression.
The results suggest that all treatments would be considered good value for money compared with no treatment from the perspective of the Australian mental health system (all ICERs < A$7000 per QALY gained, compared with a stated threshold of A$50,000 per QALY gained). Non-directive counselling was dominated by TF-CBT (more expensive and less effective) and TF-CBT plus SSRI appears more cost-effective than TF-CBT alone. However, results were sensitive to variation in the clinical effectiveness parameters, and the analysis was limited, particularly by the narrow cost perspective.
Cognitive–behavioural therapy: children who have been physically abused
Three studies106–109 examined the effectiveness of CBT specifically with children who had experienced physical abuse.
Description of studies
Participants
Age
The mean ages of children in the studies by Kolko107,108 and Runyon et al.109 were 8.6 years and 9.88 years, respectively. Adolescents in the LeSure-Lester106 study were aged 12–16 years.
Gender and ethnicity
The 12 participants in LeSure-Lester106 study were African American males, 28 of 36 completers in the Kolko107,108 study were boys, and, of those who completed at least three sessions of therapy in the Runyon et al.109 study, 28 of 44 were girls.
Recruitment
In the Runyon et al. study109 all but five of the families in the final sample were referred by child protection service agencies. Those in the Kolko107,108 study were referred by Child Protective Services (CPS) caseworkers. In the LeSure-Lester study,106 participants were recruited from a group home where they had been placed by CPS.
Maltreatment
Participants in all three106–109 studies had experienced physical abuse or (four cases in the Kolko107,108) study severe or frequent forms of physical discipline with a risk of injury.
Interventions and comparison
Kolko107,108 evaluated the impact of two interventions: a manualised FT treatment for physically abused children and a manualised CBT intervention for individual children and their parent(s). In the CBT arm, children and parents received therapy from separate therapists, who implemented parallel protocols based on social learning principles that were designed to address cognitive, affective and behavioural problems. Treatment for the children addressed their perceptions of family stress and their environments; training to develop coping and self-control skills; and interpersonal effectiveness. The intervention for parents focused on their views on violence and physical punishment, their attribution style and expectations, self-control and contingency management. There was throughout an emphasis on teaching intrapersonal and interpersonal skills. The FT was based on Belsky’s227 interactional or ecological model approach to child maltreatment. It sought to ‘enhance the cooperation and motivation of family members by promoting understanding of coercive behaviour and by teaching the family positive communication skills and how to solve problems together’ (p. 326).227
The control group received routine community services from providers not associated with the project, as mandated by family service workers. Services were based on an extensive risk assessment and included ‘home visits to provide support and information, family skills specialists who taught homemaking and related skills, and parenting information and support groups’108 plus regular telephone contact from the caseworker (p. 326).
Runyon et al.109 assessed the added value of providing treatment to children [combined parent–child cognitive–behavioural therapy (CPC-CBT)], as well as parents (parent-only CBT). Children in the CPC-CBT arm received an intervention covering psychoeducation; affect regulation; coping skills; cognitive coping; assertiveness skills and anger management; general safety skills; application of skills; development of a personal safety plan; role perspective-taking skills; problem-solving skills; preparing a letter of praise; developing a TN; and agreeing and sharing a joint TN (with parents). In addition to the usual parents’ programme (see Comparisons), parents in the experimental arm received input on parent training with the child; refinement and rehearsal of personal safety plans with the child; attention to abuse clarification and the development of the joint TN plus coaching in parent–child interactions; behaviour rehearsal of coping skills; parenting skills and safety plan; sharing of TN; and abuse clarification.
The parent-only CBT control arm received an intervention that comprised disclosure of the referral incident, engagement and assessing parents’ goals, motivational interviewing and commitment to no violence (two sessions), followed by psychoeducation and an introduction to anger management skills (two sessions), continuation of coping skills (three sessions), review and applications of skills, including ABCs of parent–child interactions, development of personal safety plans (three sessions); review of ABCs of parent–child interactions and integration/generalisation of skills (four sessions) and parent training (parent only, two sessions).
LeSure-Lester106 provided CBT to groups of two to three participants. The intervention was designed to teach participants the feelings associated with anger and aggression, relaxation and self-talk, and alternative ways of coping. It comprised a three-stage education and training module, beginning with education relaxation techniques. This was followed by education and GE and then education and anger control. The therapist used vignettes (as the basis for discussions) that reflected the participants’ natural environments, that is, the residential setting. Participants in the control arm received ‘traditional indirect therapy’ consisting of open-ended discussions and communications of the participants’ self-reports of activities and current events in their daily lives. The therapists were said to have exercised warmth, empathy and genuineness – no specific coping skills were taught.
Number and duration of sessions
Each intervention in the Kolko107,108 study entailed at least 12 1-hour weekly clinic sessions over a 16-week period, for a total of about 18 hours of service. Those in the routine community services (control) arm received variable amounts of help, ranging from services delivered on a once or twice per week basis for an indefinite period of time, to treatment plans comprising up to 20 hours of home-based service per week for up to 3 months.
In the Runyon et al.109 study, parents in the ‘combined intervention’ attended 16 2-hour group sessions over a 16- to 20-week period. Groups were initially conducted concurrently for the first hour and 45 minutes, with the last 15 minutes involving joint parent–child sessions based on families’ needs. The balance between concurrent and joint time shifted over the course of the intervention so that in sessions 12–16 the joint sessions lasted 60–75 minutes. Parents in the ‘Parent only’ condition received a similar CBT parenting intervention, but spent more time discussing the implementation of behaviour management strategies. Parents in the ‘Combined’ arm spent less time on parent skills training and more time preparing their ‘clarification letter’ (a letter that demonstrated that they took full responsibility for their abuse behaviour) and preparing for, and interacting with, their children in joint sessions. This was one of only a few interventions that included the non-offending parent.
In the LeSure-Lester106 study, participants in both arms received 26 weeks of traditional indirect therapy, and were then randomised to continue with that therapy or to receive 26 weeks of CBT. Both therapies were delivered for 1 hour, twice per month.
Outcomes and measures used in studies of cognitive–behavioural therapy for physically abused children
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Only Runyon et al.109 assessed the impact of intervention on PTSD, using the KSADS-PL268 (see Post-traumatic stress disorder). This was the primary outcome in this study.109
Behaviour
Runyon et al.109 and Kolko107,108 both used the CBCL260 to assess change in child behaviour, along with the Child Conflict Index (CCI).688
LeSure-Lester106 used a rating system of behavioural appropriateness developed within the residential setting, and comprising aggression towards peers, towards staff and compliance with house rules (all scored low or high).
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of cognitive–behavioural therapy for physically abused children
Sequence generation
Kolko107,108 used a computer-generated procedure based on Efron’s biased coin toss and was assessed as low risk of bias. Runyon et al.109 used a computer program to randomly determine the treatment type for each group, and on that basis was deemed low risk of bias. No information was provided by LeSure-Lester106 and so this study was judged unclear.
Allocation concealment
The LeSure-Lester study106 was judged as ‘high risk’, as the author was both the provider of the intervention and the researcher. The project co-ordinator in the Runyon et al.109 study was blind at pre-treatment so we concluded that allocation was concealed at this point (low risk of bias). Kolko107,108 provided no information and so was judged unclear risk of bias.
Blinding of participants and personnel
High risk: participants and personnel were not blinded in any of the three trials.106–109
Blinding of outcome assessors
Runyon et al.109 stated that the project co-ordinator (who conducted all assessments) ‘remained blind, to the extent possible, to condition assignment’ but it is not clear to what extent this was achieved. The determination of unclear risk of bias was made. Risk of bias was high in the study by LeSure-Lester106 as outcome data were provided by staff in the residential care home. Kolko108 reported that ‘[P]roject staff administering the assessment protocol were unaware of the treatment conditions to which participants were assigned’ and was judged low risk of bias, although the same data were self-reported.
Incomplete outcome data
Attrition in the Kolko107,108 study was around 6% (one case) in the FT arm, 20% (five cases) in the CBT arm and 17% (two cases) in the routine community services groups. Although these cases did not appear to be different in characteristics, the differential attrition between the two active treatment arms resulted in a judgement of high risk of bias (the reasons for dropout might be related to the intervention). No attrition occurred in the LeSure-Lester106 study, so this was judged as ‘low risk’. The Runyon et al.109 study was judged as ‘high risk of bias’: 25% children who completed 3 weeks’ therapy did not provide data post test, and their pre-test scores were carried forward. Attrition increased at follow-up and the authors present no means or SDs at this assessment point.
Selective outcome reporting
The LeSure-Lester106 study was assessed as low risk for selective outcome reporting. This study106 is small, modest in design and implementation, and data are given on those outcomes that the intervention was seeking to address (using ‘home-made’ measures). Kolko107,108 and Runyon et al.109 appear to have reported on all outcomes covered in the methods section, but, in the absence of a published protocol for either study106 both were assessed as unclear for this domain.
Other sources of bias
LeSure-Lester’s106 study was compromised by the conflation of roles held by the author, that is, researcher and therapist.
Results: cognitive–behavioural therapy for physically abused children
Meta-analysis was not possible across these studies,106–109 which are summarised narratively.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
The Runyon et al.109 study reported significant pre- to post-improvement on the total number of PTSD symptoms among all children, with the adjusted mean post-test scores for those in the combined CBT parent and CBT child group being significantly lower than those for the parent-only CBT group.
Child depression
In the Kolko107,108 study, children’s reports on the CDI80 indicated a significant reduction in severity of depressive symptoms over time [χ2 = 16.01(3); p < 0.001], but reports were said to be ‘generally low and similar across time, indicating no significant group differences’108 (p. 333).
Child behaviour
As measured by the Youth Self-Report (YSR) of the CBCL, children in all three groups in the Kolko107,108 study (CBT, FT and routine services) reported a significant reduction over time in both internalising symptoms [χ2 = 33.54(3); p < 0.0001] and externalising symptoms [χ2 = 12.26 (3); p < 0.002], with both CBT and FT showing most change on these measures. No effect was found for social competence.
Parent report on the CBCL indicated lower ratings of serious internalising behaviours over time (p < 0.07), particularly for the two treatment arms. Parents reported a significant reduction in externalising behaviour over time [χ2 = 9.53(3); p < 0.02]. Based on an inspection of the means over time, CBT appeared to show the greatest initial change and FT the greatest change at follow-up (1 year) compared with routine community services, which showed minimal change during that period.
A significant interaction was reported on the CCI [χ2 = 13.12(3); p < 0.04], reflecting the greatest decrease in scores for CBT. This measure (scored by telephone interview with the parent) estimates the presence or absence of common individual behavioural or emotional problems commonly displayed in boys or girls within the previous 24 hours.
Runyon et al.109 reported significant pre- to post-improvement in internalising and externalising scores (CBCL) for the CBT parent-only condition.
LeSure-Lester106 reported greater rates of behaviour change from pre-test to post-test for the six adolescents who received CBT. Using the rating system used by staff within the home, these six adolescents demonstrated greater rates of behaviour compliance (t = –5.64; p < 0.001) and less aggression towards staff (t = –4.56; p < 0.001) and other residents (t = –5.64; p < 0.001).
The effectiveness of cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have been physically abused
Summary
We identified only three studies106–109 of CBT interventions for children and young people who have been physically abused. Each focused on children of somewhat different ages, from middle childhood to adolescence. One study107,108 compared a CBT intervention for children and their parents with systemic FT; one study109 compared a CBT intervention for parents with one that included a parallel intervention for children, and the third study106 compared a small group version of a CBT provided to African American adolescents living in a group home as a direct result of their maltreatment, compared with non-directive group discussions.
Although very different, the three CBT interventions106–109 shared some common characteristics, namely a focus on children’s thoughts, feelings and behaviour. There was a marked psychoeducational component in both the Runyon et al.109 and the LeSure-Lester106 study, aimed at helping children to recognise and understand the consequences of abuse, and develop appropriate coping and problem-solving skills, including the development of skills to minimise risk of abuse107,108 or personal safety plans.109
The three106–109 studies are all extremely small and the overall quality is, at best, moderate in relation to risk of bias. Together with the fact that we can summarise the evidence only narratively, considerable caution is required in interpreting the data. All three106–109 studies report improvement in children’s internalising and externalising behaviour problems (common sequelae of physical abuse), but one109 of the studies found an improvement in externalising behaviour in the parent treatment group only.
The one109 study examining PTSD reported a reduction in symptoms in all children, with the most significant reduction occurring for those where both parents and children received CBT. Depression, examined in one109 study, reduced over time in both the experimental and comparison groups.
Both CBT and FT generally outperformed routine community services, resulting in greater reductions in children’s externalising behaviour and on child-reported parent-to-child violence and parent-reported child-to-parent violence.
Completeness and applicability
All three106–109 studies were conducted in the USA. Two107–109 studies were concerned with families who had come to the attention of services because of maltreatment, but where the child remained in the home. They aimed to minimise the risk of further physical abuse and to address the adverse consequences of past abuse and current maladaptive parenting. In Kolko107,108 maltreatment was judged as ‘mild/moderate’ in 87% cases, although almost half the families had children that had been hit with an object, and 50% had children who had been smacked with an open hand. In the Runyon et al.109 study, 53% families had a substantiated allegation of physical abuse or had acknowledged the use of excessive physical punishment (e.g. 65% had hit their children with an object). Many of the children in these three studies would be subject to a child protection plan in the UK, and in the Runyon et al.109 study (although not in the Kolko107,108 study) the inclusion criteria required children to have either four PTSD symptoms or an elevated score (T score of ≥ 65) on at least one externalising behaviour subscale on the CBCL.260 In this study, siblings were included in the treatment as long as they too could meet these criteria and were a focus of child protection workers’ concerns. These studies106–109 therefore have relevance to the UK context, although they are limited in their scope and the evidence base is sparse.
LeSure-Lester106 evaluated an intervention that was designed specifically to address the aggressive behaviours of boys who had been removed from the family home as a result of maltreatment. Although the focus on addressing maltreatment-related aggression is highly relevant to the UK context, the study106 says little about the intervention, and the outcome measures focus on resident–staff interaction (with an emphasis on compliance) and peer–peer violence within the home. Although the intervention is reported to have made a significant impact, the size of the study,106 plus the absence of measures or time periods to indicate the generalisability or likely maintenance of reported behaviour change, mean that its applicability is limited.
Quality of the evidence
Studies of CBT for children who have been physically abused are few in number, poorly reported, and overall of limited quality, although poor reporting may account for many of the identified risks of bias.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of CBT were located for children who have been physically abused.
Cognitive–behavioural therapy: children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
Nine randomised trials of CBT or enhanced CBT interventions were identified110–112,114–117,120,121 that were designed to address the consequence of maltreatment, irrespective of maltreatment type. The studies were themselves heterogeneous, and fall into four broad categories:
- risk reduction interventions to reduce human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) and sexually transmitted infections (STIs) among abused and neglected young people110
Description of studies
Location
All but four111,112,116,120 studies were conducted in the USA. The Rushton et al.116 study was conducted in the UK, the Farkas et al.120 study in Quebec, Canada, the Church et al.111 study in Peru and the Jensen et al.112,113 study in Norway.
Sample size
Five110,111,117,120,121 studies recruited and randomised individual participants who had been maltreated. The Church et al.111 study recruited just 16 participants, whereas the studies by Farkas et al.120 and Scheck et al.121 recruited, respectively, 40 and 60 participants to their studies of EMDR and the Shirk et al.117 study randomised 43 adolescents. Champion and Collins110 randomised 559 adolescent women.
The Jensen et al.112,113 study randomised 156 parents. (Only 135 parents participated in the study.)
The remaining three studies recruited participant pairs. Rushton et al.116 recruited 38 adoptive families. One study by Linares et al.115 enrolled 94 children, with the intervention targeted at foster parent/biological parent pairs, whereas the other Linares et al.114 study recruited 63 biological/foster parent pairs.
Participants
Age
Three114–116 studies focused on children aged < 10 years. Children in the Rushton et al.116 study were between 3 years and 7 years 11 months at recruitment; the Linares et al.115 study recruited foster parents caring for children aged 5–8 years, and children in the Linares et al.114 study were aged 3–10 years.
Five110,111,117,120,121 studies recruited adolescents. The studies by Champion and Collins110 and Church et al.111 recruited adolescent women aged 14–18 years and 12–17 years, respectively. Shirk et al.117 recruited adolescents aged 13–17 years. The two120,121 EMDR studies recruited adolescents aged 13–17 years120 and 16–25 years.121
Jensen et al.112,113 recruited the caretakers of children and young people aged 10–18 years.
Gender
Six112,114–117,120 of these studies recruited both male and female children or their carers. Some had a preponderance of one gender, for example the samples in the studies by Shirk et al.117 and Farkas et al.120 were largely female (85% and 74%, respectively). Participants in the Church et al.111 study were all male, whereas in the studies by Champion and Collins110 and Scheck et al.121 the participants were all female.
Referrals
The Rushton et al.116 study recruited adoptive parents referred from English local authorities that had high rates of adoption. Adoptors were eligible if at least one of their adopted children scored above a certain threshold on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), completed by either the adoptor or the child’s social worker, or both. The Champion and Collins110 study recruited participants from women seeking health care at a district health clinic.
Participants in the Linares et al.115 study were drawn from community-based mental health services, but it is not clear how they were recruited. Linares et al.114 recruited foster parents from one child welfare agency.
Church et al.111 recruited young men who were resident in a residential treatment refuge (Peru). Adolescents in the Shirk et al.117 study had been referred to an outpatient department in a large, urban mental health centre, and those in Jensen et al.112,113 were children referred to one of eight community clinics via normal referral routes [general practitioner (GP), Child Protection Services] who had experienced a traumatic event and who scored ≥ 15 on the Child PTSD Sympton Scale (CPSS).286
One121 of the EMDR studies recruited volunteers from adverts in a range of agencies,121 whereas the other120 took referrals only from youth protective services.
Maltreatment type
In Linares 2006,114 children had experienced physical abuse or neglect, but (by chance) only neglected children were allocated to the control condition, compared with 71% in the intervention group.
In the Linares 2012,115 children had officially substantiated histories of child maltreatment: 77% were neglected and 23% were abused either physically (18%) or sexually (5%). Some children experienced more than one form of maltreatment.
Children in the Church et al.111 study had a history of physical, psychological or sexual abuse or neglect/parental abandonment. The majority of participants in the Champion and Collins110 study (76%) had histories of sexual, physical and emotional abuse. This study110 recruited women with abuse histories or histories of STIs (because of the over-representation of maltreatment in the histories of adolescents) and was designed to ‘provide a study sample of adolescents with both a history of STI and abuse’110 (p. 142).
Participants in both EMDR studies120,121 had histories of maltreatment. Most of those in the Farkas et al.120 study had been referred to Youth Protective Services for a variety of forms of parental neglect or abuse, although some were referred for reasons of serious behaviour problems. Most participants had been referred for, or had experienced more than one form of, maltreatment; it was not possible to identify the proportion of participants who had not been maltreated. A total of 90% of participants in the Scheck et al.121 study reported being victims of physical or emotional abuse as a child, and over half of the traumas reported related to traumatic sexual experiences, such as rape or child molestation.
Adolescents in Jensen et al.112,113 had been exposed to a range of traumas, including physical and sexual abuse, and witnessing violence.
Interventions and comparisons
The interventions in the studies by Rushton et al.116 and Linares et al.114,115 were modified versions of Webster-Stratton’s Incredible Years Program (IY).212
Linares et al.114 used the manualised, group-based Parents and Children Basic Series Program (IY, Webster-Stratton et al.210) plus a coparenting intervention delivered on individually to biological and foster parent pair and target child, and which focused on learning about each other, practising open communication and negotiating interparental conflict. Therapists used family systems strategies, such as joining, didactic lesson, re-enactment and restructuring.
Linares 2012115 used a subset of the 18 IY manualised lessons contained in the Dina Program for Young Children. Modules were Understanding and Detecting Feelings; Detective Wally Teaches Problem-Solving Steps; and Tiny Turtle Teaches Anger Management, plus a lesson developed for the project and designed to promote a sense of belonging to this foster home – My Homes, My Families.
In both114,115 of these studies, foster carers in the control group received ‘usual services’.
Rushton et al.116 used the IY programme as a basis for a cognitive–behavioural programme tailored to the needs of adoptive parents, placing an emphasis on the need to conduct daily play sessions with the child and to help adopters when their child rejects their praise or their rewards. First and last sessions were focused, respectively, on getting to know the parents and introducing the programme, and reviewing progress and ending. Other sessions focused on using positive attention to change behaviour; the value of play for establishing positive relationships; using verbal praise; rewards; learning clear commands and boundaries; using ‘ignoring’ to reduce inappropriate behaviour; defining for the child the consequences of undesirable behaviour; ‘time out’ and problem-solving. Adoptive parents in the control group received an educational approach designed by an adoption adviser ‘to improve adopters’ understanding of the meaning of the children’s current behaviour and help them see how past and present might be connected’116 (p. 532), thereby helping adopters to respond more appropriately to challenges.
Church et al.111 provided a brief, single-session exposure therapy entitled emotional freedom techniques (EFT), comprising certain components of CBT and exposure therapy combined with a somatic component, having therapists or participants tap their fingers on prescribed acupuncture points. Those in the control group received no treatment.
Champion and Collins110 provided a theory-based [AIDS Risk Reduction Model (ARRM)209] CBT intervention designed to reduce risk-taking behaviour – Project Image (PI). PI is described as ‘grounded in knowledge of the target population’s behaviour and culture . . . Emphasis is placed upon understanding and dealing with male-female power relationships in African-and Mexican American culture’110 (p. 144). The intervention began with a physical examination (for STIs, etc.) followed by an enhanced counselling session (addressing adherence to medication, other treatments, sexual activity, etc.). Intervention participants were then offered two workshop sessions, 1 week apart, followed by group work and further individual counselling. The workshops and group work described have a strong psychoeducational component and a tailored skills component. Control group participants received the physical examination, abuse and enhanced clinical counselling at baseline, plus a follow-up physical examination at the end of the intervention.
Shirk et al.117 evaluated a modified CBT intervention (m-CBT) that combined CBT elements (mood monitoring, cognitive restructuring, relaxation training, activity scheduling and interpersonal problem-solving), with mindfulness-based strategies, such as taking a non-judgemental stance of observing, describing and tolerating trauma-related emotions and cognitions (Linehan et al.689). The effectiveness of m-CBT was assessed in relation to usual care (UC), in which therapists agreed to use, with control group participants, the treatment strategies and procedures that they regularly used and believed to be effective in their clinical practice).
The EMDR intervention in the Scheck et al.121 study consisted of two treatment sessions of 1 hour, 1 week apart. EMDR followed the standard protocol devised by Shapiro.690 In this study,121 EMDR was compared with an active listening intervention.
In Farkas et al.120 study, EMDR was combined with motivation–adaptive skills–trauma resolution (MASTR), aimed at addressing conduct problems (Greenwald691), motivational interviewing and a range of cognitive–behavioural training and coping skills development. MASTR is a trauma-focused treatment package that was developed for use with adolescents with conduct problems, which ‘addresses treatment obstacles by establishing sense of safety within therapy, encouraging clients to be the agents of their change, improving motivation and guiding them towards progressive successes to their goals’120 (p. 128). Participants received 12 weekly sessions of 1.5 hours of MASTR/EMDR therapy. They also continued with other forms of individual (14%), family (14%) and group therapy (29%). In this study of EMDR, Farkas et al.120 used a ‘routine care’ control group in which participants were exposed to a variety of alternative therapies.
Jensen et al.112 described the TF-CBT programme that they use as a ‘trauma specific treatment consisting of psychoeducation, learning relaxation skills, affective modulation skills, cognitive coping skills, working through the TN, cognitive processing, in vivo mastery of trauma reminders, and enhancing safety and future developments, coupled with the parental component’112 (p. 6). The parental component looked to improve parenting skills and was also used to demonstrate for the parent each treatment component that was provided to the child. Those in the control group received ‘the treatment they (TAU Therapists) considered most suitable in each individual case’112 (p. 6). Almost half of the TAU therapists described their theoretical orientation as psychodynamic, 30% as cognitive behavioural, and around 25% as family/systemic (percentages rounded up). In 35 of the 52 completed TAU cases, parents were involved in some way in more than three sessions of the child’s therapy.
Comparisons
Number and duration of treatments
The IY or IY-based interventions used in the studies by Rushton et al.116 and Linares et al.114,115 were delivered in 12 weekly sessions of 2 hours.
Church et al.111 provided one, 2-hour, single session of brief EFT.
The intervention described by Champion et al.110 comprised one ‘extensive’ individual session for physical examination and a semistructured, one-on-one interview/enhanced counselling at the outset (1.5–2 hours), followed by two workshop sessions of between 3 and 4 hours, a follow-up visit (for screening, pregnancy testing and STI treatment, if necessary) and three to five sessions of support group work followed by two or more individual sessions.
The intervention evaluated by Shirk et al.117 was designed to provide 12 weekly sessions to be delivered over a 16-week period but adolescents could continue with treatment beyond the 16-week study assessment. The same was true for the TAU group.
The TF-CBT intervention in Jensen et al.112,113 comprised 12–15 individual sessions.
In Scheck et al.,121 EMDR was delivered in two sessions, 1 week apart, and in the Farkas et al.120 study it was provided in 12 weekly sessions (duration unspecified).
Where relevant, the number and duration of comparison treatments was similar to those of the experimental intervention.
Outcomes and measures used in studies of cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Jensen et al.112,113 used two measures of PTSD. The first was the CPSS,286 a self-report questionnaire developed for children aged 10–18 years, which examines post-traumatic stress symptomatology described in the DSM-IV (criterion B, re-experience; criterion C, avoidance; and criterion D, hyperarousal).692 The second was the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for Children and Adolescents, a structured clinical interview that assesses the frequency and intensity of the 17 DSM-IV-defined PTSD symptoms.304,305
Scheck et al.121 and Church et al.111 assessed the impact of intervention using the Impact of Events Scale (IES279). Both used the total score; Church et al.111 also report outcomes for the memories and avoidance subscales.
Farkas et al.120 used two measures of PTSD. First, the relevant module of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC693) and, second, the TSCC,325,328 to assess trauma-related difficulties.
Scheck et al.121 also used the Penn Inventory for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PENN334), a self-report scale that measures symptom severity.
Depression
Jensen et al.112,113 used the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire694 to assess depressive symptoms, as this measures the full range of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for depressive disorders, and includes items ‘reflecting common affective, cognitive, somatic features of childhood depression’ (p. 361).
Shirk et al.117 and Scheck et al.121 used, respectively, the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II320) and the BDI332 to assess the impact of EMDR on depression.
Anxiety
Scheck et al.121 used the state subscale of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI333) to measure the impact of EMDR on anxiety.
Jensen et al.112,113 used the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED)307 to measure anxiety symptoms. SCARED is a self-report questionnaire with 41 items covering five specific anxiety disorders: panic disorder or significant somatic symptoms, generalised anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder and school avoidance.
Behaviour
Rushton et al.116 and Jensen et al.112,113 used the SDQ.308 Jensen et al.112,113 also used visual analogue scales to assess how far an individual child progressed on emotional distress, misbehaviour and attachment. Rushton et al.116 relied on adopter report, whereas Jensen et al.112,113 used YSR.
In Linares 2012,115 foster parents completed a six-item measure compiled from the CBCL 5-18 aggression subscale,294 and classroom teachers completed a seven-item measure compiled from the 38-item Sutter–Eyberg Student Behaviour Inventory-Revised (SESBI-R311).
The intervention evaluated in Linares 2006114 was designed to reduce externalising behaviour, and its effectiveness was assessed using three measures, and drawing on foster parent-report and biological parent-report; the CBCL;294,309 the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory-Revised310 and the SESBI-R.311
Farkas et al.120 used the parent version of the CBCL,269,294 alongside modules of the DISC to measure conduct disorder (CD) and oppositional defiant disorder.
Risky behaviour
In line with the aim of the intervention, Champion and Collins110 assessed new incidents of STI as a dichotomous variable (yes, no) at off-site, problem or scheduled follow-up visit at 6 and 21 months.
Self-control
Linares 2012115 used a 51-item measure of self-control, developed for this study and administered to foster parent and teacher using parallel versions.
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
Sequence generation
We judged three112,116,121 studies to be at low risk of bias. In the Rushton et al.,116 study adoptive parents were randomised independently by the clinical trials unit using permuted block randomisation. Jensen et al.112,113 state that a computer-generated randomised block procedure was used, and Scheck et al.121 used envelopes filled with papers labelled either EMDR or active listening (AL). These were then shuffled before being numbered 1 through 100. Envelopes were opened (consecutively) during interviews with participants, which took place after the collection of baseline data, thereby identifying to which therapy the participant was allocated.
Linares 2012115 state that children were consecutively identified, assessed and randomly assigned within agencies, but no further information was provided on sequence generation or allocation concealment.
The studies by Champion and Collins,110 Church et al.,111 Linares 2006,114 Farkas et al.120 and Shirk et al.117 provide no information on sequence generation and were judged to be of unclear risk of bias.
Allocation concealment
None of the RCTs included provided adequate information on allocation concealment, although Rushton et al.116 used a clinical trials unit to randomise participants, so all were judged as being of unclear risk of bias. The remaining eight110–112,114,115,117,120,121 studies provide no information on allocation concealment and were therefore judged unclear risk of bias.
Blinding of participants and personnel
We judged all studies110–112,115–117,120,121 as being of high risk of bias because no participant or personnel were blinded.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Rushton et al.116 make clear that blinding at follow-up interviews was not possible because involvement in the treatment was the focus of questions. It was therefore assessed as high risk.
Five110–112,114,115 studies were assessed as low risk. In both studies by Linares et al.,114,115 the authors state that intervention and assessment teams were assembled to keep interviewers blind to group assignment. Church et al.111 state that data were scored off-site and blind to the statistician. Champion and Collins110 state that group status was revealed only at the end of follow-up interviews.
Jensen et al.112,113 state that the assessments were computer assisted and conducted by an independent clinician who was blinded to the treatment conditions.
Shirk et al.117 state that post-treatment assessments were made by an independent evaluator, but the depression measure used (BDI) relies on self-completion, and so the study was assessed as being of unclear risk of bias.
In both of the EMDR studies120,121 the authors state that assessors were blind, but the measures used were largely self- and parent-report, so we judged this as being of high risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
There were no missing data in the studies by Church et al.,111 Rushton et al.116 or Linares 2012,115 which were therefore judged to be of low risk of bias. Linares 2006114 suffered attrition but reasons for attrition were largely the same (moved, discharged), although more parents in the intervention group refused to provide data post treatment and at follow-up than in the usual services group (eight vs. one). The authors also analysed the data on ITT principles and, overall, we judged the study114 to be of low risk of bias. Attrition in the Jensen et al.112,113 study was similar across the two arms, and there were no significant differences between the retention group and the attrition group, other than that the attrition group was significantly older than the retention group and the attrition group reported being exposed to significantly higher numbers of different traumatic events. The authors explore reasons for attrition and they are methodical in their analyses, undertaking both ITT analyses and per-protocol analyses, and completed case analyses (defined as those participants who completed at least six sessions). Overall we judged the Jensen et al.112,113 study to be of low risk of bias.
In the Champion et al.110 study, data are presented for only for 318 out of 409 women at 6-month follow-up (78% unadjusted) and 333 women at 12-month follow-up (81% unadjusted). Given the intervention and the participants, this level of attrition is impressively low, but it was deemed sufficiently large to warrant a judgement of high risk of bias.
Farkas et al.120 was also judged high risk of bias, primarily on the grounds of significant attrition. Of 65 adolescents randomised in this study, 15 dropped out by post treatment and a further eight by the 3 months’ follow-up. More young people dropped out of the experimental group than control group during treatment (10/33 vs. 5/32); of these, 2 of the 10 dropped out for reasons related to the treatment (refused to discuss their traumas) and two because they ceased to be in the custody of YPS and their families stopped their participation. All but one of the remaining participants dropped out because they ‘changed their minds’.
In the Scheck et al.121 study there was considerable attrition post treatment that was not accounted for in the results. We judged this to be high.
Data for 7 out of 43 randomised participants were missing at follow-up in the Shirk et al.117 study [four in m-CBT and three in UC]. Investigation led the authors to conclude that no systematic bias had occurred in attrition, and they conducted their analyses on ITT principles. We therefore assessed this study as low risk of bias.
Selective outcome reporting
Three110,112,116 studies were registered with ClinicalTrials.gov: Champion and Collins110 (NCT01387646); Rushton et al.116 (NCT04448012) and Jensen et al.112,113 (NCT00635752).
Champion and Collins110 report findings for the primary outcome (STIs at 12 months) but, in the paper identified for this review, have not yet reported on secondary outcomes (substance use, experience of abuse and frequency of unintended pregnancies). Overall, we judged this as ‘unclear’ risk of bias. The studies by Rushton et al.116 and Jensen et al.112,113 report on all primary and secondary outcomes and were therefore judged as ‘low’ risk of bias.
In general, the remaining studies appeared to indicate that they were reporting on all predetermined outcomes. However, in the absence of study protocols it is difficult to assess the risk of selective outcome reporting. Therefore, all studies were assessed as ‘unclear’ risk of bias.
Other sources of bias
None were identified.
Results: cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
No meta-analyses were possible for data from these studies,110–112,114–117,120,121 the results of which are therefore reported in narrative form.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Jensen et al.112,113 report a mean effect (ITT analyses) of treatment condition on child PTSD (measured by the CPSS) at time 3 (T3), some 7.5 months after treatment began: children in the TF-CBT group scored significantly lower at T3 [mean (M) = 11.34, SD 10.52] than participants in the comparison group [M = 16.87, SD 11.49; d = 0.51, t(154) = 3.30; p = 0.001; with Holm adjustment p = 0.006]. The authors also report a significant time by group interaction effect (F(2) = 5.01; p = 0.007; with Holm adjustment p = 0.037). Both groups showed reductions in PTSD from pre- to post-therapy assessments, and a main effect of treatment condition on functional impairment such that trauma influenced daily functions significantly less (indicated by higher scores) in the TF-CBT group (M = 10.33, SD 1.99) than in the TAU group (M = 9.22, SD 2.09) at the end of therapy [d = –0.55, t(154) = –3.32; p = 0.001; with Holm adjustment p = 0.006]. They also report a main effect of treatment on time in both groups. Analyses of completed cases yielded similar results.
Statistically significant between-group differences were found by Church et al.111 in favour of the intervention (EFT). One month after pre-test, participants who had received the intervention demonstrated a statistically significant decrease on both the total score for the IES279 and the two subscales (memories and avoidance). All participants had scored in the clinical range at baseline, and control participants remained in the ‘moderate clinical’ range post test, in contrast to those in the intervention group, none of whom was in the clinical range post test.
Analyses of PENN post-test scores in the Scheck et al.121 study indicated a significant difference in favour of the EMDR group [F(1,55) = 6.03; p = 0.02]. A similar result was found for the IES [F(1,57) = 9.93; p = 0.002].
Post treatment, Farkas et al.120 reported significant improvements in the experimental group (MASTR/EMDR) compared with control group participants for PTSD symptoms as measured by DISC [F(1,40) = 6.05; p = 0.05]. Significant improvements were also reported for the TSCC325 (trauma-related difficulties) on each of six subscales: stress, anger, depression, dissociation, anxiety and sexual concerns.120
Depression
Jensen et al.112,113 found a main effect of treatment condition on children’s depressive symptoms, with participants in the TF-CBT group (M = 14.40, SD 13.67) scoring significantly lower than those in the TAU condition (M = 22.67, SD 16.24) at T3 [d = 0.54, t(154) = 2.79; p = 0.006; with Holm adjustment p = 0.018].
Scheck et al.121 reported a significant effect for EMDR on depression [F(1,58) = 5.39; p = 0.024].
Shirk et al.117 reported significant reductions over time in BDI depression scores but no between-group differences.
Anxiety
Jensen et al.112,113 found no main effect of treatment condition on children’s anxiety symptoms. Participants in the TF-CBT group [d = 0.30, t(150) = 1.47; p = 0.114; with Holm correction p = 0.114]. Analyses of the SCARED subscales found a main effect only for generalised anxiety disorder. Completer analyses produced similar results.
Scheck et al.121 reported a significant effect for EMDR on STATE anxiety [F(1,57) = 4.89; p = 0.031].
Behaviour
Rushton et al.116 found no significant differences in child problems between the two groups at 6 months’ follow-up, although a significant difference (p < 0.007) was found for ‘satisfaction with parenting’ in favour of the intervention group (effect size d = 0.7).
Jensen et al.112,113 found a main effect of treatment condition on the SDQ (interpreted as general mental health problems). Participants in the TF-CBT group had significantly lower scores (M = 11.95, SD 6.51) than those in the TAU group (M = 14.54, SD 6.12) at the end of therapy [d = 0.45, t(152) = 2.46; p = 0.015; with Holm adjustment p = 0.030]. Completer analyses produced similar results.
In Linares 2012,115 the authors report that physical aggression decreased over time for both groups (IY, UC) but there were no between-group differences. After adjusting for gender, ethnicity, initial diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and study site, children in the UC group showed more improvement than those in the IY training group on foster parent reports of physical aggression. Rates of improvement were highest among children in the UC condition. Teachers reported no differences.
In Linares 2006,114 intervention children were reported as having lower CBC externalising T scores [F(1,97) = 2.71; p = 0.10] and Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory (ECBI) total T score [F(1,94) = 2.30; p = 0.13] at follow-up but these were not statistically significant.
Farkas et al.120 reported significant differences on the CBCL260 in favour of MASTR/EMDR for externalising behaviour [F(1,40) = 9.77; p = 0.05], but not for internalising behaviour.
Risky behaviour
Champion 2012110 reported a significant impact of the intervention, with those who had received the theory-based (ARRM209) CBT intervention experiencing fewer infections at intervals of 0–6 months (0% vs. 6.6%; p = 0.001), 6–12 months (3.6% vs. 7.8%; p = 0.005, 95% CI 0.001 to 0.386) and 0–12 months (4.8% vs. 13.2%; p = 0.002, 95% CI 0.002 to 0.531).
Self-control
Linares 2012115 report a main effect in relation to foster care reports of higher levels, and steeper rates of improvement in relation to self-control, in favour of the control group (UC).
Self-esteem
Scheck et al.121 reported a significant effect of EMDR for the TSCC335 [F(1,57) = 4.573; p = 0.04].
Parent–child relationships
Emotional Freedom Questionnaire (EFQ) scores among control children in the Rushton et al. study116 remained unchanged, whereas they were more positive at all time points for the intervention group; however, these were not significant when controlling for baseline scores.
Although not directly relevant, Linares et al.114 reported a significant difference between the intervention and UC group on coparenting flexibility [F(1,104) = 4.14; p < 0.05], coparenting problem-solving [F(1,102) = 6.38, p < 0.01] and coparenting total [F(1,97) = 5.13; p < 0.05]. This was a key aim of the study114 and was likely to have a positive impact on the experience of children in foster care.
Effectiveness of cognitive–behavioural therapy for children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
Summary
Four112,114–116 of the nine studies that we identified focused on helping caregivers (including birth parents, adoptive or foster parents) to address the consequences of maltreatment, by enhancing their knowledge and skills and providing support. Unsurprisingly, most of these studies provided services to carers of children aged < 10 years, but one112 study recruited those caring for older children. The studies compared modifications of the IY parenting programme with either TAU114 or, in the only UK study,116 with an educational approach.
The other five110,111,117,120,121 studies provided services directly to maltreated young people aged > 12 years. They included two120,121 studies that compared EMDR with alternative treatments (as usual), and three110,111,117 studies comparing modified forms of CBT or CBT ‘plus’ with either no treatment, UC or a modified form of UC.
Overall, within the range of different studies included in this section, there was some reduction in symptoms PTSD and depression with treatment. Results regarding improvement in children’s behaviour vary between the studies. One115 study targeted physical aggression and self-control in foster children, and found no differences between children whose foster parents had participated in the IY programme and those who had received UC. Indeed, when appropriate adjustments were made, those in the control group did rather better than those in the experimental group. No differences were found between the behaviour of children whose adoptive parents received a CBT parenting programme based on IY, although these parents were significantly more satisfied with parenting and were less likely to use negative parenting approaches than those in the control group.116
Conflicts between foster parents and biological parents contribute to placement instability, and work against reunification. One114 study examined an intervention designed to improve relationships between parents, foster parents and children, and enhance the consistency of parenting across the two homes. The results of this study were very positive, and have relevance to the UK context.
One110 study, focusing specifically on risky behaviour by girls, showed reduction in rates of STIs.
It is difficult to draw conclusions about EMDR, as in one120 study this was a very different intervention to the standard protocol, and it was compared with another treatment arm that was quite intensive.120
Completeness and applicability
Only one study was conducted in the UK,116 and one study was conducted in Norway.112 Both studies112,116 focused on children in a family context, and aimed to improve outcomes for children who had experienced very serious maltreatment. In the UK study, participants were recruited from social welfare agencies that were responsible for supporting adoptive parents. In Norway, the settings were trauma clinics, that is, routine community settings.
Webster-Stratton’s IY programme has been endorsed as an evidence-based parenting programme in the UK, and the applications evaluated in three114–116 of these studies have relevance to the needs of children in foster and adoptive care in the UK. The application of this programme to some of the challenges of fostering maltreated children is highly relevant, but the findings are mixed and the evidence base is limited.
Quality of the evidence
Most studies are small and there are few of them. Their heterogeneity (in terms of interventions, participants and outcome measures) prevented us from combining data in meta-analyses, and therefore the results from this part of the review should be treated with caution.
Overall, the quality of the evidence relating to studies of CBT for children who have experienced a range of forms of maltreatment is moderate, largely because of the impact of lack of information, which, if available, might demonstrate enhanced quality – or the reverse.
Economic evidence
One economic evaluation,613 carried out in the UK, explored the cost-effectiveness of two parenting programmes, including a cognitive–behavioural approach, for adoptive parents. The study613 used data from the Rushton et al.116 RCT, described above, and compared the two parenting programmes (a cognitive–behavioural approach and an educational approach), which were combined due to small sample sizes (n = 19) to services as usual (n = 18). The intervention was delivered to adoptive parents of children who were adopted at between the ages of 3 and 8 years, who were screened for serious behavioural problems early in the placement.
Cost-effectiveness was explored in terms of the primary outcome measure of the study, the SDQ, and, additionally, in terms of parent satisfaction, shown to be more effective in the parenting programmes than service as usual. Resource use included health, social care and specialist educational services, as well as the use of the parenting programmes, which were costed using nationally applicable unit costs. Costs were expressed in 2006–7 pounds sterling (£). No discounting was applied to costs and effects because of the short time horizon of the study, with follow-ups carried out post treatment (approximately 12 weeks after study entry) and 6 months post treatment (approximately 9 months after study entry).
At the 6-month post-treatment follow-up, costs were significantly higher for the parenting programmes and there was no significant difference between the two groups on the SDQ. However, parental satisfaction was significantly higher for the combined parenting programme group. Thus, in terms of the primary clinical outcome, service as usual was found to dominate the parenting programmes (less expensive and no difference in outcomes), whereas for parental satisfaction, the authors report an ICER of £337 per unit improvement in satisfaction. The authors conclude that the parenting programmes may be cost-effective in enhancing parental satisfaction. However, the study613 was severely limited in a number of important ways, which would caution against such a conclusion. In particular, sample sizes were extremely small and thus the results are unlikely to be adequately powered. In addition, the significant results were based on the only secondary outcome measure to show a significant difference in favour of the intervention, suggestive of a post hoc analysis, and no exploration of uncertainty was undertaken.
Economic analysis: cognitive–behavioural therapy
Given the effectiveness evidence presented showing promising benefits of CBT for sexually abused children, it was thought appropriate for consideration to be given to the development of a decision-analytic model to more fully explore the cost-effectiveness of CBT in this population. As described above, however, only one relevant economic evaluation of CBT for sexually abused children was located, a decision model based on Australian data with cost data limited to the cost of CBT only, showing cost-effectiveness advantages for CBT. One further economic evaluation of CBT, focusing on children who have experienced different types of maltreatment, was located, but this trial-based study was limited by small sample sizes and showed no economic advantage for CBT in terms of the primary clinical outcome. In the absence of any other better quality UK-based data, a decision model was ruled out.
Instead, we conducted cost-effectiveness analyses of CBT for sexually abused children using PTSD, anxiety and depression outcomes combined with intervention costs, calculated as described in Chapter 2. For PTSD and anxiety, we used SMDs. For depression we used CDI scores,80 reported in five studies. Given the sensitivity of the effectiveness results for some outcomes to the use of change scores, suggesting baseline imbalance, outcome data were calculated using random-effects meta-analyses for mean change from baseline and assuming a correlation between baseline and follow-up of 0.5. Correlation was varied between 0 and 1 in sensitivity analysis but this did not alter the results, so only the results for a correlation of 0.5 are reported. All results are reported for outcomes post treatment (the time point with the greatest amount of data) and at 12-month follow-up (the time point at which the advantage for CBT is the smallest, thus a more conservative approach). Analyses were repeated for all maltreatment types, but this did not alter the results so they are not reported here.
Results
Table 11 reports the cost and outcome parameters and the deterministic and probabilistic ICERs for SMD and CDI outcomes. ICERs are the additional cost per unit change in SMD, for PTSD and anxiety, and the additional cost per unit change in CDI score, for depression. In all analyses, the CBT group are associated with higher costs and better effects than the control group. In addition, effectiveness advantages for CBT compared with the control group are always greater post treatment than at 12-month follow-up, resulting in larger ICERs at 12-month follow-up (larger expenditure needed to generate a unit improvement in outcome).
For the SMD outcomes, Figures 9 and 10 show the cost-effectiveness plane for both PTSD and anxiety outcomes, post treatment and at 12-month follow-up, respectively. The cost-effectiveness plane is used to illustrate differences in costs and effects between different strategies, in this case CBT and the control. It consists of four quadrants, for which the x-axis represents the additional level of effectiveness generated by one intervention compared with another and the y-axis represents the additional cost of one intervention compared with another. The scatter points on the cost-effectiveness plane represent multiple cost and effectiveness pairs generated by the probabilistic sensitivity analysis, but for ease of interpretation, can be viewed as pairs of individuals, one receiving CBT and the other in the control group. Points that fall in the north-west quadrant represent the situation in which CBT is more expensive and less effective and thus dominated by the control group. For those in the south-east quadrant, CBT is more effective and less expensive and thus dominates the control. For those in the north-east quadrant, CBT is more effective but also more expensive, and, for the south-west, CBT is less effective but also less expensive; both of these quadrants involve a trade-off between costs and effects.
As only the cost of CBT was considered in the current analysis, all points fall above the x-axis (costs higher for the CBT group than the control group). In terms of effects, although the SMD results from the meta-analyses suggest advantages for CBT rather than the comparison, this advantage is no longer evident in the probabilistic analysis (involving assigning probability distributions to costs and effects, as outlined in Chapter 2). Instead, differences in effect are relatively equally distributed to both the right of the y-axis (effects better for CBT) and the left (effects better for control).
Associated uncertainty is displayed in the CEACs in Figures 11 and 12, which illustrates the probability that CBT is more cost-effective than the control, for different levels of willingness to pay for additional benefits. The CEACs suggest that the probability of CBT being more cost-effective than the control post treatment does not rise much above 50% for PTSD outcomes and remains below 50% for anxiety outcomes post treatment (see Figure 11). The results are similar at 12-month follow-up (see Figure 12).
Figures 13 and 14 show the cost-effectiveness plane for CDI outcomes post treatment and 12-month follow-up, respectively. Again, as only the cost of CBT was considered in the analysis, all points fall above the x-axis (incremental costs higher for the CBT group than the control group). In terms of effects, the results are similar at 12-month follow-up to those for SMD outcomes, showing no clear advantage for CBT compared with the control. The post-treatment results, however, suggest some effectiveness advantage for CBT, with a larger proportion of points falling to the right of the y-axis (effects better for CBT) than the left (effects better for control).
Uncertainty is illustrated by the CEACs reported in Figures 15 and 16 for post-treatment and follow-up outcomes, respectively. The post-treatment results suggest that the probability of CBT being more cost-effective than the control reaches approximately 90% for willingness-to-pay values of around ≥ £5000. Using the 12-month follow-up data, however, the results are similar to those for SMD outcomes and do not rise much above 50%.
Relationship-based interventions
Studies covered in this section focus on interventions that seek to improve relationships between children and their parents (RBIs). They include those designed to promote secure child attachment and the positive outcomes associated with that, and parenting interventions designed to improve the quality of parenting of maltreating parents, thereby bringing about positive benefits to children who have already experienced maltreatment.
In total, we identified 15 controlled studies122–140 that assessed the effectiveness of RBIs. The studies are grouped as follows:
Because of the imbalance of numbers, we are not dealing with each subset of interventions entirely separately, but, where appropriate, we group them for descriptive and reporting purposes. We do not comment on gender in this section, as, by definition, all RBIs address the relationship between parents and their children, irrespective of gender. Details of child gender, where reported, are available in Chapter 3.
Description of studies
Study designs
The Becker-Weidman134,135 study was a COS. The remaining studies122–133,136–140 were randomised trials.
Location
All studies were conducted in the USA123–129,131–136,140 except for Moss et al.130 (Quebec, Canada), Hughes and Gottlieb139 (Eastern Canada) and Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 (Australia).
Sample sizes
Controlled studies ranged from small to a moderately large sample size; the smallest sample size was 60 (with only 46 ultimately included in the analyses)125,126 and the largest were 151137,138 and 210.131 See Chapter 3 for further information.
Participants
Attachment-orientated interventions
Age
Five of the attachment-orientated studies focused on infants up to 24 months of age. Infants in the Bernard et al.122 study were aged between 1.7 and 21.4 months at enrolment (mean 10.1 months), and those in Dozier et al.125,126 were aged 3.6–39.4 months [note: two papers report the results of this study: one125 deals with the ‘first 60 children who completed the experimental or control intervention’ (p. 773) and reports on cortisol levels and behaviour problems; the second study126 reports on ‘the first 46 children who completed the experimental or control intervention’ (p. 5) and reports on attachment behaviours]. In both Cicchetti trials,123,124 infants had a mean age of just over 13 months. Spieker et al.131 recruited mother–infant dyads, for which the infants were aged 10–24 months.
Four studies recruited older children.127–130,132,133 Preschoolers in the Toth et al.133 study had a mean age of 48.2 months (SD 6.88); in the Sprang132 study the mean age was 42.5 months (SD 18.6 months); children in Lieberman 2005127–129 were aged 3–5 years, and in the Moss et al.130 study they were between 12 and 71 months.
In the Becker-Weidman134,135 study, children’s ages ranged from 5 to 16 years.
Referral
In the Dozier et al.125,126 study, foster parents were referred at the time of initial infant placement (presumably by child welfare staff). Consent from both birth parents and foster parents was required. In the Bernard et al.122 study parents were referred by agencies working with Child Protection Services. In the Sprang132 study parents were referred for a relational intervention following a university-based assessment.
In Cicchetti 2006,123 a recruitment liaison officer was retained in the Department of Human Services to identify all infants who were known to have been maltreated or who were living in maltreating families with their biological mothers. The same method was used in Cicchetti 2011,124 Spieker et al.131 and Toth et al.133 Spieker et al.131 used a liaison officer to identify infants of ‘an appropriate age who had experienced a court-ordered placement that resulted in a change of primary caregiver with the prior seven weeks’ (p. 5). Toth et al.133 used a liaison officer to identify families with a preschool-aged child with a documented history of maltreatment.
Participants in the Lieberman et al.127–129 study were referred to the study by paediatric providers, family resource programmes, child-care providers and child protection workers when there were clinical concerns about the child’s behaviour. Those in the Moss et al.130 study were referred by welfare or community services.
Becker-Weidman134,135 used data from cases closed in 2001 or 2002 in which children had received a diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder and there was a significant history of physical abuse, emotional abuse or neglect, sexual abuse or institutional care. One group comprised 34 children who had received DDP and another group of 30 children who received UC.
Parent–child interaction therapy interventions
Age
Children in the PCIT studies were aged 4–12 years in the study by Chaffin et al. 136 In the studies conducted by Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck, all but three of the children in the 2011137 study were age between 2.5 and 7 years, and in the 2012138 study the authors report a mean age of 4.57 (SD 1.3) years.
Referral
In the PCIT trials, referrals came from welfare workers in the Chaffin et al.136 study and from a variety of sources, including self-referral in the Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 studies. Eligibility in the Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 studies depended on being assessed as at high risk for child maltreatment, using a semistructured interview designed to identify proximal risk factors such as high levels of parental distress, aggressive patterns of communication and use of inappropriate discipline strategies.
Parent-focused interventions
Referral
Eligible families were identified by child protection agency staff as in need of parent training in the Hughes and Gottlieb139 study. Cases were not necessarily on the Child Abuse Registry. In the study by Valentino et al.,140 families were recruited from the Department of Child Services, which provided families with information from flyers and from individual case workers.
Maltreatment
Attachment-orientated interventions
In each of the attachment-focused studies, the intervention was directed at a mother–infant dyad. Five123,125,126,131,132 studies focused on children in out-of-home placements as a result of maltreatment. The remaining five122,124,127–130,133 studies focused on children living with their biological parents.
Dozier et al.125,126 included young children newly placed in foster care. Apart from children placed at birth, these children would have experienced neglect or abuse prior to placement. In Sprang132 the children were in foster care, having experienced ‘severe maltreatment’ (p. 82) with an attachment disorder that threatened to disrupt the placement. Children in the study by Cicchetti 2006123 were also in foster care as a result of maltreatment, with disorganised attachment. Spieker et al.131 recruited toddlers who had experienced a recent, court-ordered placement and their caregivers. Children in foster care were participants in the COS.134,135
In Cicchetti 2011,124 infants who were known to have been maltreated or who were living in maltreating families with their biological mothers were identified for recruitment. All forms of maltreatment were included. In the recruited sample, almost 72% of infants had directly experienced abuse and/or neglect during the first year of life: 83% of infants had been neglected and 69% had been emotionally maltreated. None of the infants had been sexually abused. Over half of the infants had experienced more than one type of maltreatment.
In Bernard et al.122 the infants in were in families where there was a risk of out-of-home placement for a variety of reasons, including domestic violence, parental substance use, homelessness and child neglect.
In Moss et al.130 the majority of primary caregivers (72%) had been reported for child neglect: 7% of primary caregivers were reported for physical abuse and 3% of primary caregivers for sexual abuse; 16% of children had been both physically abused and neglected, and 2% of children were both neglected and sexually abused.
Toth et al.133 recruited families with a preschool aged child with a documented history of maltreatment. Almost 60% of the children had experienced more than one form of maltreatment; 21% had experienced neglect; and 14% had experienced emotional maltreatment. Two children had been sexually abused.
Lieberman et al.127–129 recruited child–mother dyads where the child had been exposed to marital violence (confirmed by mother’s report on the Revised Conflict Tactics Scale398), when the father figure perpetrating the violence was no longer in the home and there were concerns about the child’s behaviour or mother’s parenting.
Parent–child interaction therapy
In the Chaffin et al.136 study, children were referred for concerns about physical abuse. In both studies by Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck,137,138 families were referred for physical and emotional maltreatment or neglect. Children with a history of sexual abuse were excluded, as PCIT is contraindicated for this form of maltreatment. Parents could self-refer and were accepted if the pre-assessment interview revelaed previous experience of a parenting intervention, a high risk of child maltreatment and high levels of child behaviour problems.
Interventions and comparisons
Attachment-orientated interventions
The included studies122,125,126,130,132 covered four interventions.
Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up
Dozier et al.,125,126 Sprang132 and Bernard et al.122 assessed the effectiveness of ABC (see Appendix 5). In the Dozier et al.125,126 study, parent trainers were professional social workers or psychologists with at least 5 years’ experience; in the Sprang132 study, they were social workers, psychiatrists or psychiatric nurse practitioners. Bernard et al.122 used parent trainers who had experience of children and strong interpersonal skills.
Dozier et al.125,126 and Bernard et al.122 compared ABC with an educational intervention borrowed partly from the home visitation component of the early intervention programme developed by Ramey et al.,695,696 which was designed to enhance cognitive and, especially, linguistic development. Components that involved parental sensitivity to child cues were excluded specifically to keep the interventions distinct.
In Sprang,132 ABC was compared with a wait-list control in which participants accessed the bi-weekly support group for parents that was also accessed (separately) by the intervention group.
Moss et al.130 assessed the effectiveness of an unnamed short-term attachment intervention designed to promote maternal sensitivity and child attachment. Mothers in both arms of the trial received services as usual (comprising a monthly visit by a child welfare caseworker), but only mothers in the experimental group received the attachment intervention. Bernard et al.122 describe the intervention provided by Moss et al.130 as one based on their own ABC intervention combined with interventions developed by Backermans-Kranenburg et al.219 and Moran et al.220 The intervention was provided in home, using video feedback, by experienced clinicians with at least a bachelor’s degree in psychology, who received training from attachment experts.
Child–Parent Psychotherapy, Pre-school–Parent Psychotherapy and Infant–Parent Psychotherapy
In three studies, Cicchetti et al.123,124,133 evaluated the effectiveness of each age-related version of this therapy with another manualised home-based intervention. Lieberman et al.127–129 also assessed CPP.
Toth et al.,133 Cicchetti 2006123 and Cicchetti 2011123 compared CPP with a psychoeducational parenting intervention (PPI), and referred to as psychoeducational home visiting in Toth et al.133 and a management-as-usual group, in which families received services typically available to maltreating families in the community. PPI/psychoeducational home visitation (PHV) was based on the home visiting programme developed by Olds et al.,216–218 augmented by ‘a variety of cognitive and behavioural techniques in order to address parenting skill deficits and social-ecological factors, such as limited personal resources, poor social support, and stresses in the home associated with maltreatment’ (p. 794). The interventions were provided by trained, master’s level therapists, on a weekly basis over the course of 1 year.
Lieberman et al.127–129 compared CPP with individual psychotherapy plus case management.
Promoting First Relationships
Spieker et al.131 evaluated PFR, a manualised, infant mental health training programme, aimed at early years’ professionals. This formed the basis of an intervention programme tailored to the needs of children in care who had experienced disrupted placements. PFR was compared with early education support, a home visiting intervention aimed at connecting families to community resources and suggested activities to promote development.
Dyadic developmental psychotherapy (see Appendix 5)
Becker-Weidman134,135 compared DDP with TAU – essentially assessment and, for just over half, another form of treatment.
Parent–child interaction therapy
In Chaffin et al.,136 a variety of therapists (including basic trainees, experienced trainees and experts) delivered PCIT (see Appendix 5) alone or in enhanced form (i.e. with the addition of services targeting family-specific problems, such as domestic violence, substance abuse or parental depression). Participants in the control group took part in a standard community-based parenting group.
Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2011137 compared time-variable PCIT (TV/PCIT) with an attention only wait-list control in which parents were contacted weekly for brief conversations regarding family or other concerns for 12 weeks. In TV/PCIT parents are coached during the Child Directed Interaction phase (CDirI; see Appendix 5) until mastery criteria were achieved for two consecutive sessions, before moving onto parent-directed interaction (PDI) phase.
Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2012138 compared standard PCIT (S/PCIT), in which participants received only 12 coaching sessions, regardless of proficiency, with an attention wait-list control. In this study the authors also used their data to compare the effectiveness of S/PCIT with TV/PCIT by drawing on the data available from their earlier trial.
Parent-focused interventions
The intervention in Valentino et al.140 (reminiscing and emotion training, RET) focused on encouraging parents to engage in elaborative and emotionally supportive reminiscing about positive and negative everyday past events as a means of increasing parental sensitivity and addressing multiple developmental sequelae of maltreatment. Sessions were led by bachelor-level family coaches, and included the use of video feedback and daily homework. In Hughes and Gottlieb,139 the intervention was the Webster-Stratton IY parenting programme, a standardised, video-based, modelling intervention based on social learning theory and tailored to the developmental needs of families with young children. The group facilitator was the first author of the study. Both Hughes and Gottlieb139 and Valentino et al.140 compared the experimental intervention to a wait-list control.
Number and duration of sessions
Attachment-orientated interventions
Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up
In the studies by Dozier et al.,125,126 Bernard et al.122 and Sprang,132 both interventions (experimental and control) were provided in 10 weekly, hour-long sessions, based on a structured training manual.
Child–Parent Psychotherapy, Pre-school–Parent Psychotherapy and Infant–Parent Psychotherapy
Parents receiving both IPP and CPP (see above) in the Cicchetti 2006123 study received weekly home visits over a 12-month period, with an average of 21 sessions conducted in the IPP group and 25 sessions conducted in the IPP group. In the Cicchetti 2011124 study, participants in the CPP and IPP arms also received weekly home visits over a 12-month period, with the average number of visits being approximately 46 for the CPP group and 49 for the IPP group.
The CPP intervention in Lieberman et al.127–129 was delivered weekly for 50 weeks, with each session lasting approximately 60 minutes. Most dyads attended a mean of 32 CPP sessions; those receiving individual psychotherapy had, minimally, monthly phone calls from a case manager (who they could also contact if needed) plus information and referral to mental health clinics of their choice. Face-to-face meetings were scheduled when clinically indicated. Most mothers received individual treatment (77%) and 55% of children also received individual treatment.
Recipients of PPP were seen for weekly 60-minute dyadic sessions over a 12-month period. Those receiving PHV received a similar ‘dose’.
In the Moss et al.130 study, the manualised intervention consisted of eight home visits, of approximately 90 minutes, once a week.
Promoting First Relationships
PFR was delivered in 10 weekly sessions of 60–75 minutes in the home.131 Those receiving early educational services received 3-monthly 90-minute, in-home sessions delivered by an early education specialist.
Parent–child interaction therapy
As indicated above, in the 2011 Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck study137 parents receiving TV/PCIT received as many sessions of CDInt as was necessary to achieve the prescribed mastery criteria, before proceeding to PDI. On average, PCIT participants who completed treatment engaged in a total of 11.8 sessions and five PDI coaching sessions. In Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2012,138 parents in receipt of S/PCIT received 12 sessions of coaching only, irrespective of whether or not they had reached the mastery threshold for progression from CDInt to PDI.
Outcomes and measures used in studies of relationship-based interventions
Attachment
Three122,123,130 studies assessed the impact of the intervention on attachment using the Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure.336
Dozier et al.125,126 asked foster parents to record infants’ behaviour when distressed and in the presence of their primary caregiver using the Parent Attachment Diary (PAD697). Spieker et al.131 used the Toddler Attachment Sort-45,350 a modified version of the Attachment Q-sort698 to assess children’s attachment security, and Becker-Weidman134,135 used the Randolph Attachment Disorder Questionnaire.364
Internal working models
Toth et al.133 specifically explored the effectiveness of two developmentally informed preventative interventions on children’s internal representations of self, and of self in relation to other, using a narrative story stem task from the MacArthur Story Stem Battery (Bretherton I, Oppenheim D, Buchsbaum H, Emde RN & the MacArthur Narrative Group, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1990, unpublished).
Child behaviour
Six130–132,134,135,137,138 studies examined the impact of attachment-based interventions on child behaviour, using the CBCL.260
In addition to the CBCL,260 Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2011137 and 2012138 assessed the impact of PCIT on child behaviour using the ECBI (parent report),310 and Spieker et al.131 did so using the Brief Infant Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA352) and selected items from the Bayley III Screening Test.355 Chaffin et al.136 used the Behavior Assessment System for Children.365
Hughes and Gottlieb139 used the Child Autonomy Observational Scale to assess child autonomy. Again, this tool was developed for the study, based on the theoretical underpinnings of Deci and Ryan.699,700
Dozier et al.125,126 assessed the impact of the intervention on children’s behaviour using the Parent Daily Report (adapted from Chamberlain and Reid458).
Child stress
Dozier et al.125,126 and Cicchetti et al.124 used analyses of cortisol to assess levels of stress in infant participants.
Parent behaviour
The Dyadic Parent–Child Interaction Coding System (DPICS)701,702 was used by all three PCIT studies,136–138 although Chaffin et al.136 used the DPICS-II702 and Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 used the DPICS-III.701
Sprang132 and Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 examined the effect of the intervention on parents’ abuse potential using the Child Abuse Potential Inventory (CAPI366).
Parental stress
Spiekar et al.,131 Sprang132 and Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137,138 assessed the impact of the intervention on parenting stress, using the Parenting Stress Index-Short Form.342
Maternal sensitivity
The primary caregiver outcome in the Spieker et al.131 study was maternal sensitivity, assessed using a modified score of the Nursing Assessment Teaching Scale,358 the secondary outcomes being a measure of parenting support for the child, using the Indicator of Parent–Child Interaction351 and commitment to the child, assessed by answer to interview questions from This Is My Baby.359 Moss et al.130 measured maternal sensitivity using the Maternal Sensitivity: Maternal Behaviour Q-Set.339
Parenting behaviour
In the Hughes and Gottlieb139 study, a scale was developed that reflected the theoretical underpinnings of the study, namely the Parenting Skills Observation Scale (developed by the authors for this study).
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of relationship-based interventions
Sequence generation
All studies, except for Spieker et al.131 and Hughes and Gottlieb,139 were judged to be ‘unclear’ risk of bias for sequence generation because no information was provided on how the randomisation sequence was generated. Spieker et al.131 and Hughes and Gottlieb139 were assessed as ‘low’ risk. Spieker131 stated that they used a computer-generated sequence, blocked by caregiver type. Hughes and Gottlieb139 referred to a random numbers chart.
Allocation
All studies122–140 were judged unclear risk of bias on allocation concealment because of a lack of information.
Blinding of personnel and participants
All but two123,125,126 studies were assessed as unclear risk of bias for blinding of participants, predominantly due to lack of information. Dozier et al.125,126 stated that ‘foster parents and birth parents were blind to condition’ and was therefore judged low risk of bias. Cicchetti 2006123 did not blind participants but this was deemed unlikely to influence outcomes of this study because all participants in this study were infants aged, on average, 13–31 months.
Blinding of outcome assessors
All studies, except for Sprang,132 were judged as ‘low risk of bias’ for blinding of outcome assessors because assessors were blinded to the hypotheses, the assignment of participants and/or to the collected data. Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2011137 did not state that their outcome assessors were blinded, but this trial used the same procedures and outcomes measures as the 2012138 trial to which it was linked, and in the 2012 study the outcome assessors are said to be ‘blind to treatment condition’. Sprang132 was assessed as high risk as the authors relied solely on self-completed measures collected by the treatment staff at post test.
Incomplete outcome data
With the exception of Toth et al.,133 all studies were deemed to be at low risk of bias. Some studies122,136–138 had no missing data, some studies127–129,131,132 used a supplementary ITT analysis and the remaining studies123–126,130,133–135,139,140 conducted analyses of those who dropped out and those who completed treatment, and concluded that they did not differ. The Toth et al.133 study was assessed as ‘unclear’ risk of bias as missing data were not reported and analyses included only participants who completed treatment.
Selective outcome reporting
Cicchetti 2006123 was judged as ‘unclear’ risk of bias because, despite prospective registration and a (broad-brush) description of measures relevant to the domains outlined in that protocol, the paper reports only on attachment classifications. Cicchetti 2011124 was judged as ‘unclear’ risk of selective outcome reporting bias because no means or SDs were presented and only latent growth curve data.
Spieker et al.131 and Lieberman et al.127–129 also registered their trials (ClinicalTrials.gov identifiers NCT00339365 and NCT00187772, respectively). Spieker et al.131 reports on those outcomes listed in the trial registration and was judged as ‘low’ risk of bias on this domain. Lieberman et al.127–129 reports on all primary outcomes and one of two secondary outcomes. Overall we judged this study to be ‘low risk’ of bias on this domain. The secondary outcome not reported on by Lieberman et al.127–129 is child’s cognitive functioning.
All but one125,126 of the remaining studies were deemed unclear risk of bias, as although they appeared to report on all of those outcomes expected, without access to the original study protocols, we cannot be certain. Dozier et al.125,126 was assessed as ‘high risk of bias’ in light of the fact that two papers reporting the results of this study use different samples and report different outcomes without complete cross-referencing to a statement of all per-protocol outcomes.
Other sources of bias
No other potential sources of bias were identified in most of the studies. However, Sprang132 had an unclear risk-of-bias assessment because the authors reported that expectancy effects and trust in instructors may have played a role in the findings. Furthermore, Bernard et al.122 and Dozier et al.125,126 had an additional high risk of bias because the Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure was used as a measure for participants aged > 24 months, which extends beyond the age range for which it has been validated.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Risk of bias: controlled observational studies of relationship-based interventions
The quality of the COS of Becker-Weidman134,135 was variable. No attempt (or no information on any attempt) was made to blind participants or outcome assessors in this study. This study134,135 provided a clear description of its objectives, the main outcomes to be measured, the characteristics of patients included, the intervention of interest and the potential confounders. Although the main findings were adequately described, the relevant adverse events were not adequately addressed. Characteristics of participants lost to follow-up were adequately described and accounted for. It was not possible to determine whether or not the participants and treatment received were representative of the population of interest. Becker-Weidman134,135 used appropriate statistical tests and accounted for confounding variables in their analysis.
Results: relationship-based interventions
Given the heterogeneity of the interventions, we report the results as they relate to the three groupings set out above, starting with meta-analyses where available.
Attachment-focused interventions
Secure attachment
Bernard et al.,122 Cicchetti 2006123 and Moss et al.130 assessed the impact of attachment-based interventions on the security of a child’s attachment as measured by Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure. The pooled estimate using a random-effects model was 0.14 (SMD) (95% CI 0.03 to 0.70) (Figure 17).
The I2-statistic indicates that 82% of the variation in the point estimates is due to heterogeneity (p-value for heterogeneity = 0.004; τ2 = 1.52).
Disorganised attachment
Bernard et al.,122 Cicchetti 2006123 and Moss et al.130 assessed the impact of attachment-based interventions on a reduction of a child’s disorganised attachment style, as measured by Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure. The pooled estimate using a random-effects model was 0.23 (SMD) (95% CI 0.13 to 0.42; p = 0.00001) (Figure 18). The I2-statistic indicates 17% of the variation in the point estimates is due to heterogeneity (p-value for heterogeneity = 0.030; τ2 = 0.05).
Avoidant attachment
Both Cicchetti 2006123 and Moss et al.130 assessed the impact of attachment-based interventions on a child’s avoidant attachment style, as measured by Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure. The pooled estimate using a random-effects model was 0.90 (SMD) (95% CI 0.13 to 6.37; p = 0.09) (Figure 19).
The I2-statistic indicates 64% of the variation in the point estimates is due to heterogeneity (p-value for heterogeneity= 0.09; τ2 = 1.29).
Other attachment measures
Using an ANCOVA, Spieker et al.131 found no significant differences between intervention and control groups for security of attachment at either post-test or 6-month follow-up (post-test F = 0.68, d = 0.16; p = 0.410; follow-up F = 0.12, d = –0.13; p = 0.746).
Dozier et al.125,126 used an ANCOVA (intervention group by time) and found that children in the treatment group had less attachment avoidance than those in the control group over time [F(1,44) = 5.02; p < 0.05]. There was no difference for attachment security (p > 0.10).
Stress
Cicchetti et al.124 used latent growth curve analysis to examine trajectories of cortisol regulation over time, in an analysis combining data from the experimental and ‘other treatment control’ groups (i.e. CPP and PPI). The authors found divergences emerging between the three groups (maltreated intervention, maltreated community control and poor, but non-maltreated, comparison), starting half way through the intervention. Contrary to expectation, no differences were found between those maltreated infants in the intervention groups and those in the control group at baseline. Whereas infants in the maltreated group showed a steady decline in morning cortisol levels (which is elevated in normal samples) over the 2-year study period this did not occur for infants in the maltreated intervention group, for whom cortisol levels were normalised (i.e. no different from infants in the non-maltreated comparison group) and remained so at 1-year post-intervention follow-up. A similar change in cortisol secretion was also found by the Dozier et al.125,126 study, for which an ANCOVA (intervention group by time) found that children in the intervention group showed significantly lower overall cortisol levels than the control group over time [F(1,46) = 4.55; p = 0.04].
Additional results
In the Spieker et al.131 study, child competency improved post test but this was no longer significant at follow-up (post-test: F = 4.77, d = 0.42, p = 0.03; follow-up: F = 0.63, d = –0.16, p = 0.429). Child sleep problems were different between groups at follow-up but with a small effect size (d = –0.13; p = 0.09).
Toth et al.133 used a general linear model and found that, looking at the interaction of study conditions by time, and three out of six narrative variables showed improvement. The improvement was for maladaptive maternal representations [F(3,118) = 3.13; p < 0.05], negative self-representation [F(3,118) = 4.93, p < 0.01] and mother–child relationship expectations [F(3,118) = 2.72; p < 0.05].
Becker-Weidman134,135 found that at 4-year follow-up the treatment group had a statistically significantly improvement compared with the control group for security of attachment (t = –12.23; p < 0.001), withdrawal (t = –4.352; p < 0.001), social problems (t = –2.654; p < 0.05), thought problems (t = –3.505; p < 0.01), attention problems (t = –4.239; p < 0.001), rule-breaking behaviour (t = –6.733, p < 0.001) and aggressive behaviour (t = –7.104; p < 0.001), but not for anxiety/depression (t = –1.091; p = 0.28).
Parent–child interaction therapy
Child behaviour (externalising)
Both Chaffin et al.136 and Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2011137 and 2012138 assessed the impact of PCIT on child externalising behaviours (as measured by various scales). Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2012138 used a control group that overlapped with their 2011 study,137 and the extent of the overlap was unclear, so we decided only to include the data from their 2011 report of TV/PCIT in the meta-analysis to avoid the problem of double counting.
The pooled estimate, using a random-effects model, was 0.03 (SMD) (95% CI –0.38 to 0.43) (Figure 20). The I2-statistic indicates that 44% of the variation in point estimates between the two studies is due to heterogeneity, making it difficult to draw conclusions about the effect of PCIT on externalising behaviours/symptoms in maltreated children from these two studies. It is perhaps worth noting that Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2012138 also report significant improvements in the behavior of children whose parents participated in S/PCIT and, in both studies,137,138 gains were said to be greater for those who completed PCIT.
It is also worth noting that Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2012138 report a greater improvement in externalising behaviours/symptoms (p < 0.001) after treatment with S/PCIT than with wait-list controls. This study also found a greater improvement in externalising behaviours/symptoms (p = 0.002) after treatment with S/PCIT than with TV/PCIT.
Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck 2011137 and 2012138 also measured the intensity of behaviour problems using the ECBI.
Thomas 2011137 found a greater reduction in child behaviour problems (p < 0.001) and intensity (p < 0.001) after treatment with TV/PCIT than with wait-list controls.
Thomas 2012138 found a greater reduction in child behaviour problems (p < 0.000) and intensity (p = 0.019) after treatment with S/PCIT than with wait-list controls.
Thomas 2012138 also found a greater reduction in child behaviour problems (p = 0.001) after treatment with S/PCIT than with TV/PCIT. However, there was no difference between S/PCIT and TV/PCIT for child behavior intensity (p = 0.096).
Parenting interventions
In the study by Valentino et al.,140 children in the intervention group had richer memory recall (p < 0.01, d = 0.71) and made more emotion references (p < 0.001, d = 1.35) than control children during conversation with parents but not with experimenters.
Contrary to expectation, there was no effect of the Webster-Stratton IY parent programme on child autonomy in the Hughes and Gottlieb study.139
Effectiveness of relationship-based interventions for maltreated children
Summary
We identified three groups of RBIs.
The first group of 10122–135 studies addressed the problems of insecure or disorganised attachments among maltreated children. Of these, four122,125,126,132 studies evaluated the effectiveness of the short intervention ABC or based on ABC, and four123,124,127–129,133 studies evaluated an attachment theory informed intervention known as IPP. All were short-term, manualised programmes, and they focused on promoting sensitive and responsive care to children adversely affected by maltreatment, with the focus on the carer–child relationship and patterns of interaction. In only one134,135 study were children and young people seen by a therapist. In this study,134,135 the therapy, DDP, is described as a FT treatment based on attachment theory, in which the relationship between therapist and child, caregiver and child, and therapist and caregiver provide the context for treatment. The last131 study evaluated the impact of training early years professionals.
The body of evidence relating to attachment informed interventions is promising, particularly in relation to ABC and IPP interventions. Meta-analyses of data from three122,123,130 studies (two ABC, one IPP) indicate significant increases in attachment security and decreases in disorganised attachment. These results are consistent with those reported in studies, the data for which we were unable to combine in these meta-analyses, with some indication that children in the control group evidenced deterioration, that is, more developed disorganised attachment. This suggests that timely interventions may be able to prevent cumulative consequences of maltreatment.
Findings in relation to children’s behaviour were mixed, and generally did not reach the level of statistical significance. One130 study that recruited children with a wide age range (1–5 years) explored the moderating impact of age and found that reductions in child externalising and internalising problems was associated with increasing age in the intervention group, whereas a marginal increase in behaviour problems was found with age for the control group. Such analyses can only be hypothesis generating and, given the size of the study,130 probably highly speculative. However, the authors note that the maladaptive trajectories for maltreated children increasingly diverge from their non-maltreated peers over time, with the transition to school often being particularly difficult for maltreated children who maintain increasingly socially dysfunctional patterns of aggression and social withdrawal. Therefore, an intervention that has the potential to reduce behaviour problems in preschool children may well be particularly helpful. A similar pattern was noted by Dozier et al.125,126
Children’s stress levels, as measured by cortisol secretion patterns, also improved with attachment-based interventions.
We identified three randomised trials of PCIT. All were concerned with addressing behaviour problems resulting from physical abuse by helping parents change the way they interacted with their children. A meta-analysis of data from measures of child externalising behaviour indicated no effect of PCIT. Chaffin et al.136 say in the discussion that this may be because, in this study, PCIT was evaluated as a parent treatment; the study included children older than those customarily included in PCIT for child behaviour problems. Given the relational context of maltreatment, and the emphasis placed by these authors on the importance of the escalating coercive interactions, this account reads as a ‘post hoc’ explanation of an unexpected finding. There is some uncertain evidence regarding the potential reduction in child externalising behaviour problems following PCIT. Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck137 report a decrease in the severity of behaviour problems of children receiving PCIT, as perceived by parents, but these children evidence no diminution in externalising behaviour as assessed by the CBCL260 (parent report).
As intended, the RET intervention improved children’s memory recall and emotion referencing, with their parents.
Completeness and applicability
None of these studies was conducted in the UK, but all are concerned with relationship problems that are familiar to mental health professionals and social workers. It is difficult to overstate the importance of secure attachment as a public health issue, and the attachment difficulties that so often result from maltreatment present a cumulative threat to children who experience abuse and neglect. The attachment interventions evaluated are relatively brief, manualised interventions that include techniques and strategies currently in use by many health professionals in the UK, for example video interactive guidance. Given the findings reported, well-conducted trials of these interventions in the UK would be appropriate prior to recommending their widespread adoption.
There is currently no strong evidence to support the use of either PCIT or Webster-Stratton’s IY Program as an intervention to address the emotional or behavioural problems of maltreated children, and their impact on improving parenting is clinically questionable.
Quality of the evidence
Only three122,125,126,132 of these studies were judged to be of high risk of bias in any of the seven domains assessed, but most were so poorly reported that judgements of unclear risk of bias outnumbered those of low risk. Overall, these studies do better in terms of detection bias (the blinding of outcome assessors) and attrition bias (incomplete outcome data). Six122–128,132 of these studies were conducted by two American teams, and essentially replicate one another, with variations in the profile of participants. Each team is evaluating an intervention that they have helped to develop. Additional, independent evaluations of both interventions (ABC and IPP) would be helpful, together with better reporting.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of RBIs for children who have been maltreated were located.
Systemic interventions
This category includes interventions that aim to benefit the child by bringing about change within the family and other systems in which the child’s life is embedded. Altogether, we identified eight controlled studies (19 citations) that assessed the effectiveness of systemic interventions. We organised these into the following five subcategories:
- a transtheoretical intervention that integrated family systems, social learning theory and a conflict mediation perspective141
- a family-based programme for the treatment of CSA.150
In what follows, we provide descriptive data on the entire group of eight studies,107,108,141–144,147–149 separating them out only when we describe the types of intervention and the results.
Description of studies
Study design
Of the eight107,108,141–144,147–150 controlled studies, six107,108,141–144,148,149 were randomised trials. One was the QEx Bagley and LaChance study150 and one was a COS.147
Location
One150 study was conducted in in Canada. The remainder of the studies107,108,141–144,148,149 were carried out in the USA.
Sample sizes
Sample sizes were generally small. Linares 2015141 randomised 22 sibling pairs. Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 and Swenson et al.144 had samples of 81 and 90, respectively, and the remaining RCTs had sample sizes of 30143, 43142 and 55107,108, respectively. Bagley and LaChance150 had a sample size of just 65 (after attrition and exclusions) and Schaeffer et al.147 had a sample size of 25 youth–mother dyads.
Participants
Systemic family therapy
Multisystemic therapy
Age
Brunk et al.142 give the mean ages of children in each arm of the study (9.8 years MST, 6.8 years control) but no overall mean age or age range. Schaeffer et al.147 recruited families with children aged 6–17 years. Swenson et al.144 and Danielson et al.143 reported that the mean age of children was around 14 years.
Multigroup family therapy
Transtheoretical
Sibling pairs in the Linares 2015141 study were between 7.2 and 9.7 years of age, and the authors report no significant between-group differences in sibling configuration, with 26% being both males, 37% both females and 37% mixed gender.
Family-based intervention for child sexual abuse
Age and gender
Bagley and LaChance150 targeted only female victims of sexual abuse, with mean ages of 11.2 years and 11.8 years in the experimental and control groups, respectively.
Referrals: all systemic interventions
Linares et al.141 recruited eligible children and foster carers from three participating fostering agencies. Families in the other seven107,108,142–144,147–150 systemic intervention studies were recruited from CPS or similar governmental agencies. In the study by Danielson et al.,143 families were also referred or recruited from clinics/treatment providers.
Maltreatment type
Systemic family therapy
Kolko107,108 recruited families in which children had been physically abused.
Multisystemic therapy
Participants in the Danielson et al.143 study were sexually assaulted adolescents. Brunk et al.142 and Swenson et al.144 recruited families with problems of physical abuse and neglect (excluding children who had been sexually abused). Schaeffer et al.147 recruited families in which children had been exposed to abuse or neglect (within the previous 180 days) and in which parental substance misuse was confirmed or suspected by CPS.
Multigroup family therapy
Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 recruited families where there was abuse or neglect, and where sexual abuse was not the primary allegation, as this was deemed to constitute a substantially different dynamic. Two-thirds were referred for physical abuse, 29% experienced severe neglect and in 24% of cases emotional abuse was also alleged. Ten per cent of participants in this study also suffered sexual abuse.
Transtheoretical
Ninety per cent of the children in the Linares et al.141 study were in foster care because of neglect (the other 10% were missing this information on their files).
Family based
Bagley and LaChance150 recruited families in which female children had been subject to intrafamilial sexual abuse.
Sources of maltreatment
Where specified, the source of maltreatment was within the family, including biological parents, step-parents, or a parent’s cohabiting partner. In Danielson et al.143 the source of maltreatment was not reported.
Interventions and comparisons
Systemic family therapy
Kolko107,108 compared a manualised FT treatment for physically abused children to individual child and parent CBT and a control group comprising usual services. The FT was based on Belsky’s227 interactional or ecological model approach to child maltreatment.
Multisystemic therapy
Please see description of MST in Appendix 5.
In the Brunk et al.142 study, MST was compared with parent training groups. Families in the control group in the study by Danielson et al.143 received TAU in a specialist clinic utilising evidence-based interventions. Participants who received Multisystematic Therapy for Child Abuse and Neglect (MST-CAN) in Swenson et al.144 were compared with a group who received Enhanced Outpatient Treatment (EOT). EOT comprised the services usually provided by the treatment centre for physically abused young people and their parents (including individual and FT, and referral for other services, including medication); enhanced engagement (including telephone reminders, rescheduling of missed appointments, costs of transport to the Centre); and the parenting programme ‘Systematic Training for Effective Parenting of Teens (STEP-TEEN)’ – a structured, group-based programme of seven lessons that combines didactic instruction, role-play, videotapes and group discussion to equip parents with the skills needed to understand and communicate with teenagers, to problem-solve, helping parents to accept responsibility for the abuse and encourage co-operation.
Multigroup family therapy
Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 compared multifamily group therapy (MFGT) with traditional FT. MFGT included elements of family systems theory, structural FT, group therapy, behaviour modification, CBT, reality therapy, parent education, and crisis intervention. The traditional FT was described as drawing on several theoretical frameworks, including structural FT, behaviour modification and cognitive–behavioural strategies.
Transtheoretical systemic
This is the description used by Linares et al.141 to describe a family-focused programme with three components: sibling pair; foster parent; and joint sibling/foster parent. The programme is delivered by two master’s level clinicians (one working with the sibling pair, whereas the other delivers the parent sessions to the foster carers); joint sessions taking place at the beginning and end of every session. The content of sibling and parent sessions was largely skills based, with behaviour rehearsal and reinforcement. Homework and between-sessions practice were integral components. In this study141 the control group received ‘usual services’ (unspecified).
Family-based intervention for child sexual abuse
The Child Sexual Abuse Treatment Program (CSATP704) drew on Maslow’s self-actualisation theory and included IT, dyadic therapy (victim–mother, victim–sibling), group therapy (victims), and FT (victim, mother, father and available siblings). The control group consisted of those eligible for treatment but ultimately not referred by CPS, as it was thought the child was less severely affected by the abuse.
Number and duration of sessions
Systemic family therapy
Both the FT and CBT interventions in the Kolko107,108 study were provided for at least 12 sessions, of 1 hour, over 16 weeks.
Multisystemic therapy
In the Brunk et al.142 study, MST was delivered in eight, weekly, 1.5-hour sessions. In the Danielson et al.143 study, Risk Reduction through Family Therapy (RRFT) was delivered in 1- to 1.5-hour-long sessions over an average of 34 weeks.
Treatment duration in the studies by Schaeffer et al.147 and Swenson et al.144 was based on family need. In the Swenson et al.144 study, the number and duration of sessions ranged from daily sessions to one or two per week, with an additional ‘on-call’ 24-hour service for dealing with crises. On average, families availed of 88 hours over 7.6 months (range 2–12 months). In the Schaeffer et al.147 study, participants remained in treatment for an average of 285 days (range 144–365 days) with 92% being judged as having completed treatment.
Multigroup family therapy
Families receiving MGFT met with a four-person clinical team (in groups of three to four families) for a total of around 80 hours over a 34-week period. On average, most families received 20 family-to-family sessions lasting around 2.5 hours each, although this varied considerable across families. No information was available on the amount of traditional FT provided to control participants.
Transtheoretical systemic
The foster family-focused programme in the Linares 2015141 study was delivered in eight, 90-minute, weekly sessions by two master’s level clinicians (one working with the sibling pair, the other delivering the parent sessions to the foster carers), with joint sessions taking place at the beginning and end of every session. No information was available on the quantum of ‘usual service’ provided to the comparison group.
Family-based intervention for child sexual abuse
The CSATP150 included an average of 78 hours of IT, 37 hours of dyadic therapy (victim–mother, victim–sibling), 32 hours of group therapy (victims) and 14 hours of FT. No information is available on the usual services provided to the control group.
Outcomes and outcome measures: systemic interventions
Outcomes varied by intervention type and included outcomes related to child functioning and adjustment; externalising behaviours; risky sexual behaviour; substance use; internalising behaviours; and mental health, including anxiety, depression and PTSD.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Swenson et al.144 and Schaeffer et al.147 both used the TSCC.378,383 Danielson et al.705 used the University of California Los Angeles PTSD Index for DSM-IV (Adolescent version) (UCLA-A) and Caregiver version.374
Depression
Danielson et al.143 and Kolko107,108 both assessed the impact of intervention on depression using the CDI.299
Bagley and LaChance150 used the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D369).
Behaviour problems
Kolko 1996,107,108 Swenson et al.144 and Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 assessed changes in problematic behaviour using various reporting forms and versions of the CBCL YSR.198,269,706,707 Danielson et al.143 used the Behavior Assessment System for Children-Second Edition365 and Bagley and LaChance150 relied on parent, social worker and self-report of problem/delinquent behaviours. Brunk et al.142 used the Behavior Problem Checklist (BPC; Quay and Peterson, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, 1975, unpublished).
Kolko107,108 also assessed child conflict with the CCI688 and hostility using the Children’s Hostility Inventory.301
Self-esteem
Bagley and LaChance150 used the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES no reference provided by the authors).
Child functioning
Kolko 1996107,108 assessed overall child functioning with the Kiddie Global Assessment Scale (KGAS) (no reference provided by the author) and assessed peer relationships using The Friendship Questionnaire.300 Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 used the Children’s Action Tendency Scale CAS385 and the Index of Peer Relations386.
Substance use
Danielson et al.143 used the Time Line Follow Back Interview376 and urine drug screens to assess the impact of intervention on substance use and risky behaviour.
Other outcomes measured by studies of systemic interventions
Maltreatment
Repeat abuse or high-risk parental behaviours were measured by Kolko,107,108 Swenson et al.,144 and Schaeffer et al.147 Child abuse potential was assessed with the CAPI366 by Kolko107,108 (CAPI366) and Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 (CAPI361,708). Schaeffer et al.147 also reported out-of home placements.
Parental functioning
A number of studies assessed the impact of interventions on parental functioning.
Swenson et al.144 report on the impact of MST on parental psychiatric distress using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis 1975380).
Schaeffer et al.147 used a number of measures to assess the impact of the intervention on ‘key risk factors and indices of child maltreatment and maternal substance use147 (p. 599). These included the Addiction Severity Index-Fifth Edition,384 the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II272) and the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS302). Linares 2015141 used a modified version of the Child Conflixt Index370 to assess the conflict resolution skills in their study with foster parents.
Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 incorporated measures of social support (Social Support Index387) parental problem solving (Problem-Solving Inventory388), attitudes towards child rearing (Adult-Adolescent Parenting Inventory709), and knowledge of child development (using a 30-item measure designed for the study).
Brunk et al.142 used the BSI371 to assess parental functioning.
Family functioning an adjustment
Kolko,107,108 Brunk et al.142 and Danielson et al.143 each used the FES (respectively citing372,375,710).
Kolko107,108 also used two additional measures: the FAD711 and the Conflict Behavior Questionnaire.226 Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 used the Family Assessment Form (FAF).390
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of systemic interventions
Figures 31–32 and Appendix 10 provide a summary of the risk of bias of studies of systemic interventions.
Sequence generation
Risk of bias in sequence generation was judged low in three trials: Kolko107,108 used a computer-generated procedure based on Efron’s biased coin toss; Swenson et al.144 used a computer-generated table of random numbers; and Danielson et al.143 randomised participants using computerised blocked randomisation.
The remaining RCTs141,142,148,149 were judged to be ‘unclear’, as the only information available was that participants were ‘randomised’.
Allocation concealment
None of the RCTs included provided adequate information on allocation concealment and so all were judged as being of unclear risk of bias.
Blinding of participants and personnel
With the exception of Swenson et al.,144 who stated clearly that participants were not blinded, no study referred to the blinding of participants or personnel. Given the nature of the intervention, it is unlikely that blinding was possible in any of the studies, so we judged these to be high risk of bias.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Blinding of outcome assessors was carried out in the studies by Brunk et al.142 and Linares et al.,141 so these studies were classed as being of low risk of bias. The use of self- and parent-report measures in the studies by Danielson et al.,143 Meezan 1998148,149 and Swenson et al.144 resulted in a judgement of ‘high risk of bias’, and also in Kolko 1996107,108 in which self-report measures undertaken at the beginning of the FT sessions with the family all present as a potential source of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
We judged the risk of bias as a result of missing data to be low in three trials.141,143,144 These trials minimised potential bias by maintaining a high retention rate and clear reporting144 or using ITT analysis.141,143 Attrition in Kolko107,108 was around 6% (one case) in the FT arm, 20% (five cases) in the CBT arm and 17% (two cases) in the routine community services groups. The differential attrition between the two arms resulted in a judgement of high risk of bias (reasons for dropout might be related to the intervention). Brunk et al.142 and Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 were also judged to be of high risk of bias. Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 took no account of attrition and analyses were based on available case data, resulting in a judgement of high risk of bias. Dropouts in Brunk et al.142 were evenly distributed across the two arms of the trial, but reasons for drop out are not given, and the authors do not report main effects. We therefore deemed this trial high risk of bias.
Selective outcome reporting
Three142,144,148,149 studies were assessed as being of high risk of bias: Swenson et al.144 did not report means and SDs for pre and post measures at each time point, and effect sizes were missing for some measures, leading to high risk of bias; Brunk et al.142 do not report data on main effects or provide post-treatment means, with insufficient data for effect size calculations, and, although stating that they measured child behaviour problems using the BPC (Quay and Peterson, 1975, unpublished), they present no results for this measure. Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 did not report data for ‘non-significant’ results, leading to high risk of bias.
Linares 2015141 was assessed as unclear, as, although there is no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then not reported, there is no published protocol for this study.
The study by Danielson et al.143 was judged to be of ‘low risk of bias’: the authors report on the primary outcome measure specified in the trial registration (ClinicalTrials.gov NCT00998153) and two of the three secondary measures of PTSD and family environment, but not on risk behaviours as measure by the Youth Risk Behavior Survey.
Other sources of bias
No other source of bias were identified.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Risk of bias: quasi-experimental and controlled observational studies of systemic interventions
The quality of the QEx study150 and the COS147 was variable; overall, the quality of the Schaeffer et al.147 study was good and that of the Bagley and LaChance study150 was adequate. No attempt (or no information on any attempt) was made to blind participants or outcome assessors in either study. Both studies147,150 provided a clear description of the objectives of the study, the main outcomes to be measured, the characteristics of included participants and the intervention. Distribution of potential confounders was adequately described in the Schaeffer et al.147 study and not described in the Bagley and LaChance150 study. Both clearly described the main findings and relevant adverse events. Characteristics of patients lost to follow-up were adequately described and accounted for in the analysis in both studies.147,150 The participants and treatment received in the Schaeffer et al. study147 was representative of the population of interest. Bagley and LaChance150 appeared to include those children who were more severely affected by the abuse in the treatment group.
In terms of the analysis, Schaeffer et al.147 and Bagley and LaChance150 used appropriate statistical tests and accounted for confounding variables in their analyses.
Results: systemic interventions
Meta-analysis was not possible across these studies, which are summarised narratively. Given the heterogeneity across studies, we report the results in the three groupings used above: FT, MST and ‘other systemic’.
Family therapy: results
The following results are these reported by Kolko.107,108
Child depression
Children’s reports on the CDI indicated a significant reduction in severity of depressive symptoms over time [χ2 = 16.01(3); p < 0.001], but there were no significant between-group differences.
Child behaviour
As measured by the YSR of the CBCL, children in all three groups (CBT, FT and routine services) reported a significant reduction over time in both internalising [(χ2 = 33.54(3); p < 0.0001) and externalising symptoms [χ2 = 12.26(3); p < 0.002], with both CBT and FT showing most change on these measures. No effect was found for social competence.
Parent report on the CBCL indicated lower ratings of serious internalising behaviours over time (p < 0.07) particularly for the two treatment arms. Parents reported a significant reduction in externalising behaviour over time [χ2 = 9.53(3); p < 0.02]. Based on an inspection of the means over time, CBT appeared to show the greatest initial change and FT the greatest change at follow-up (1 year) compared with routine community services, which showed minimal change during that period.
A significant interaction was reported on the CCI [χ2 = 13.12(3); p < 0.04] reflecting the greatest decrease in scores for CBT. This measure (scored by telephone interview with the parent) estimates the presence or absence of common individual behavioural or emotional problems displayed in boys or girls within the previous 24 hours.
The authors did not report statistical tests for children’s hostility but the presented means and SD indicate that participants receiving FT reported a small decrease in hostility scores over time.
Child global functioning
Kolko107,108 reported a significant increase in K-GAS (Global Assessment Scale for Children: Kiddie-GAS) scores over time for all children in the study, with no group differences. There was no difference between the CBT, FT and control groups in reduction over time on fears related to abuse.
Family functioning
The results of subscales for the FES and the FAD indicate more improvement over time among children and parents in the CBT and FT arms than those in routine services.
Multisystemic therapy: results
No meta-analysis was possible for MST because of the limited number of studies reporting appropriate data. For these interventions, we report the results for those outcomes that were directly related to children and young people, and not those related to parents, which includes measures of parental substance abuse,147 parenting stress,142 parental mental health,142,144,147 parenting behaviour,144,147 social support144 and re-abuse.144
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Danielson et al.143 report that a mixed-effect regression model indicated that MST youth demonstrated a decrease in parent-reported PTSD from baseline to 6-month follow-up. This was significant (p < 0.001) and the decrease was greater than that reported by those in the ‘TAU’ control group (for the difference; p = 0.004). The between-group difference for adolescent-reported PTSD was non-significant, although both groups reported improvement. In this small study,143 there was considerable baseline inequality and results of this pilot study need to be treated very cautiously.
Swenson et al.144 found a significantly greater improvement in PTSD symptoms in the MST-CAN group, with the number of youth scoring in the clinical range reducing by half (17.8% at baseline to 8.9% 16 months later) compared with enhanced outpatient treatment groups (19% at baseline and 21.4% at 16 months).
Schaeffer et al.147 did not find any change in PTSD or dissociation following treatment with Multisystemic Therapy-Building Stronger Families, but report data for only the treatment group.
Depression
Danielson et al.143 report that a mixed-effect regression model indicated that intervention youth demonstrated a decrease in CDI scores from baseline to 6-month follow-up. This was significant (p < 0.001) and the decrease was greater than that reported by those in the ‘TAU’ control group (for the difference; p = 0.008). Baseline inequality augurs caution in interpreting these results. No change was reported by Schaeffer et al.147 for youth depression.
Anxiety
Schaeffer et al.147 reported that youths whose families received the intervention experienced a significant decrease in symptoms of anxiety (medium effect size), as measured by the TSCC. Unfortunately, this study147 presents data only on pre–post intervention and does not compare this reduction with outcomes for the control group.
Child behaviour
Danielson et al.,143 based on their mixed-effects regression model, report improvements from baseline to 6 months’ follow-up for internalising behaviour in both intervention and TAU groups, but the experimental group did significantly better (p = 0.008). No between-group differences were found for externalising behaviour (improvement occurred in both groups over time).
Swenson et al.144 found statistically significant improvement in parent-reported internalising behaviour in the intervention (MST-CAN) group with no improvement evident in those receiving enhanced outpatient treatment. No differences were found between enhanced outpatient treatment and MST in externalising behaviours (measured by CBCL).
Brunk et al.142 reported measuring child behaviour problems using the BPC (Quay and Peterson, 1975, unpublished), but no results are provided for this measure.
Substance use and risky behaviour
Danielson et al.143 specifically targeted the risk of substance use and mental health problems of sexually assaulted adolescents. The authors conducted a Poisson mixed-effects regression model, which indicated greater reductions in substance use (number of days’ use from baseline to 6 months) among experimental youth than in those in the TAU control. No between-group differences were found for risky sexual behaviour (assessed by numbers of sexual partners and/diagnoses of STIs in previous 3 months).
Family functioning
Danielson et al.143 reported improvements in adolescent and parent reports of family cohesion (FES Cohesion scale) and reductions in family conflict (FES Conflict scale).
Family-based systemic interventions (including transtheoretical): results
Depression and self-esteem
Bagley and LaChance150 used measures of depression (the CES-D369) and self-esteem (RSES404) because the authors regarded them as ‘valid and reliable measures of psychological vulnerability’ (p. 208). Statistically significant gains are reported for adolescents in the intervention compared with those who did not receive the family-based programme.
Behaviour
Bagley and LaChance150 reported a reduction in problem behaviours among the sexually abused adolescent girls in this study, assessed from parent and social worker reports of at least one incident of delinquency, marked aggression in school, school dropout, suicidal behaviours, running away from home or problem sexual behaviour. Adolescents exhibiting at least one of these behaviours in the treatment group reduced from 48% to 7%. By contrast, a small increase from 33% to 40% was reported for those in the control group. The difference between groups was statistically significant (p < 0.05).
Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 reported improvements for child externalising behaviour, both for those in the intervention group (MFGT) and those in the comparison group (who received FT). The measure used was the CBCL and the difference was not statistically significant.
Physical aggression from older towards younger siblings in the Linares 2015141 study was reduced in the intervention group (p < 0.05) but no between-group differences were found for verbal aggression from older to younger siblings, or verbal and physical aggression from younger to older siblings.
Family functioning
Meezan and O’Keefe148,149 assessed the impact of intervention on family functioning using a modified version of the FAF (interview). The authors report significant improvements in the experimental group in relation to the amount of support available to them, their parent–child interactions, and the amount of stimulation available to their children. In contrast, the control group showed significant change only in relation to the support available to them. The reports give the reader the impression that the authors are interpreting the data in the most favourable ways possible.
Effectiveness of systemic interventions for maltreated children
Summary
We identified eight107,108,141–144,147–150 studies that evaluated a heterogeneous group of interventions informed by systems theory and offered to different participants. Four studies evaluated various forms of MST, comparing this with CBT,142 TAU,143 enhanced outpatient treatment,144 and Comprehensive Community Treatment.147
Of the remaining four studies, one compared systemic FT with CBT;107,108 one worked systemically with families in which a young person had been the subject of incestuous abuse;150 a third used MFGT;148,149 and the final study141 described itself as a transtheoretical intervention that focuses on three family subsystems: sibling pairs in foster care, the foster parent, and foster parent/sibling pairs.
All of these interventions included cognitive–behavioural strategies and psychoeducation, but their underlying theories of change were primarily systemic.
The four142–144,147 MST studies are heterogeneous and the results are variable. Only one144 study of the three143,144,147 MST studies assessing the impact of the intervention on PTSD reported a significant benefit in favour of MST. This rather larger study144 (n = 90) halved the percentage of youth scoring in the clinical range for self-reported PTSD symptoms from 18% at baseline to 9% at 16 months post baseline, in contrast with the group receiving enhanced outpatient services, for which the percentage increased from 19% to 21%. Retention in both the treatment and the study144 was high, and the intervention appears to have been successful at not only addressing the mental health symptoms of participating youth, but also addressing those aspects of parenting associated with maltreatment from both youth and parent perspectives, in particular reducing parental neglect and assault. The other two studies143,147 found no significant between-group differences in depression or PTSD. Schaeffer et al.147 observe that these young people reported subclinical levels of concern at baseline. Danielson et al.143 (who studied sexually abused children) hypothesise that the absence of effect is, in part, because of the small size of the study and considerable baseline inequality. In this study143 the primary outcome was substance use risk and mental health problems, and the authors report a significant reduction in substance use and associated risk factors among MST youth compared with TAU.
As indicated earlier, both CBT and FT outperformed routine community services. This early comparative study107,108 was one of the first to submit FT to rigorous evaluation and the authors note that FT has been less frequently used in the context of child maltreatment than CBT; they recommend further development, including the incorporation of a structured approach to address the parent–child relationship. At the same time, they highlight the need for CBT to give attention to how best to discuss children’s attributions of their victimisation.
There is no strong evidence of benefit to children from multigroup FT with abusive and neglectful families.148,149 Linares et al.141 report promising, but mixed results of an intervention designed to reduce sibling aggression in foster care, and this small study141 requires replication before any conclusions can be drawn. However, there is some indication that foster parents can learn strategies to minimise sibling aggression and its adverse effects on psychological child well-being.712
Completeness and applicability
All of these generally very small studies were conducted in North America. MST enjoys a strong evidence base in relation to vulnerable groups such as young offenders, and the extension to abusive and neglectful families suggests that MST-CAN has potential as an effective intervention for families whose behaviour places their children at risk of significant harm. Results of the other MST studies are less persuasive than those of Swenson et al.144 but of particular interest is the QEx study by Schaeffer et al.147 This study147 examined the impact of an MST intervention for maltreating families in which parental substance abuse was a major issue. Although this study147 showed a reduction only in child anxiety, given the numbers of children with child protection plans for whom parental substance misuse is a key factor, this intervention is potentially highly relevant, and the authors report an ongoing, large-scale longitudinal randomised trial that will allow the investigation of the programme’s effectiveness with different subgroups, that is, different substance misuse and maltreatment characteristics. MST is a costly intervention, but may be cost advantageous.
Quality of the evidence
Six107,108,141–144,148-149 of the included controlled studies were randomised trials, but the majority of risk-of-bias judgements made were ‘unclear’ as a result of poor reporting. The QEx study147 was judged good overall (but it was a small study), and the COS147 was judged adequate. Overall, the quality and extent of the evidence for systemic interventions to address the effects of child maltreatment are parsimonious.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of systemic interventions were located for children who have been maltreated.
Psychoeducation
Psychoeducation forms an important component in a range of different interventions, but those coded as psychoeducational interventions use it as their main focus. They draw heavily on social learning and cognitive theory to conceptualise and address maladaptive patterns of behaviour and beliefs that have developed as the result of exposure to abuse or neglect.
We identified 17151–168 studies that assessed the effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions.
Description of studies
Study design
Seven151–157 of the psychoeducational intervention studies were randomised trials. Trowell et al.155 was designed as an other-treatment control study, comparing a psychoeducational group with IT.
Three158–160 studies used a QEx design and the remaining seven161–168 studies were COSs.
Location
All151,152,154,156,157 but four155,158,159,165 of the psychoeducational intervention studies were conducted in North America.
One155 RCT was conducted in the UK. The remaining six151,152,154,156,157 randomised trials took place in the USA151–154 and Canada.156,157
Two159,160 of the three QEx studies were carried out in Canada and the third158 was conducted in the Netherlands.
The remaining COSs took place in the USA161,162 Canada163,164,166–168 and Spain.165
Sample sizes
All of the randomised trials made use of small to moderate sample sizes, ranging from a total of 42 participants (with only 38 participants completing post-treatment interviews)156 to 181 participants (with 174 participants completing the follow-up interview).151
Of the QEx studies, Noether et al.158 had a large sample size of 253 participants (with 210 participants completing the 12-month follow-up interview); Simoneau et al.159 and Tourigny 2007160 recruited 49 and 55 participants, respectively.
Sample size in four of the COSs was small, ranging from 27 participants161 to 42.166,167 Holland et al.164 and Hébert et al.163 had samples of 66 and 90, respectively, although data were presented for only 88 participants in the Hébert and Tourigny163 study, and Duffany and Panos162 recruited a sample of 617.
Participants
Age
Across all studies, the mean age of child participants ranged from 4.95 years152 to 14.8 years.166,167 Barth et al.161 recruited the foster parents of children aged 9 years on average.
Gender
Four studies focused exclusively on females.155,160,166–168 Of those with mixed samples, nine151,154,157,159,161–164 had a sample that was at least 50% female. Five152,153,156,158,165 studies had a sample that was at least 50% male.
Recruitment
Participants from all 17151–168 studies were recruited from a wide range of mainly government agencies, including child protection agencies,157,160,166–168 Family Services,153,164 Family Sexual Abuse Action Centre,163 Children’s Justice Centre,162 social services agencies,151,154,156 local residential homes,165 the authors’ own clinics and local agencies155 and community and shelter outreach centres.152 Noether et al. recruited participants from mothers who participated in a national, ‘longitudinal study of women with histories of violence and co-occurring substance use and mental health disorders’158 (p. 827). We have no information on the recruitment sources for Barth et al.161
Maltreatment
Five151–154,156 of the seven RCTs focused on children who had witnessed or been exposed to IPV. Graham-Bermann et al.151 reported that 30% of the children in this study had also experienced physical harm.
In Noether et al.158 (quasi-experiment), the sample was drawn from a longitudinal study of women with a history of violence, and co-occurring substance use and mental health disorders.
One155 randomised trial and two quasi-experiments159,160 focused on sexual abuse, as did five161–164,166,167 of the COSs.
One157 randomised trial and two COSs165,168 focused on children who had experienced physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse and neglect.
Interventions and comparisons
All 17151–168 studies used a group-based format of psychoeducation. Although the interventions were predominantly child focused, one intervention provided training for foster carers.161 Control group participants in all but two of the psychoeducational studies received either no intervention or management as usual. Overbeek et al.153 made use of a control programme ‘Jij hoort erbij’ (‘You belong’), based on an analysis of non-specific factors used in the specific factors intervention programme. Trowell et al.155 compared group psychoeducation with brief, focused, individual psychoanalytic psychotherapy.
Psychoeducation for children exposed to intimate partner violence
Graham-Bermann et al.151 described two psychoeducational interventions: one focused solely on children, and one combining a programme for children with a programme for their mothers. The programme is known as Kids Club.713 The child intervention sought to improve children’s knowledge about family violence, influence their attitudes and beliefs about families and family violence; and improve their emotional adjustment and social skills. The intervention for parents aimed at improving their repertoire of parenting and disciplinary skills, enhancing their social and emotional adjustment, thereby reducing the children’s behavioural and adjustment difficulties. Howell et al.152 evaluated a preschool version of the combined parent-and-child intervention described by Graham-Bermann et al.151 In this version the children’s intervention was designed to promote social competence and the mothers’ programme to improve their social and emotional adjustment. In both studies, children were from families through which they had been exposed to IPV.
Overbeek et al.153 developed a child’s psychoeducational intervention based on Graham-Bermann’s Kids’ Club713 but with some topics amended (e.g. more time spent on identifying, differentiating and dealing with emotions) and added (e.g. secrets, contact with the violent parent, and the future). Traumatic experiences are directly addressed in order to prevent avoidance of the topic and provide children with a sense of mastery. The intervention covered affective modulation, emotion regulation skills, coping and processing, social skills and enhancing future safety and development. Overbeek et al.153 developed parallel parent sessions independently from Kids’ Club,713 for which the focus was on psychoeducation, improving parenting and disciplinary skills, and helping parents accurately interpret children’s feelings and behaviour, providing them with emotional support.
In the Sullivan et al.154 study, children attended a psychoeducation group (The Learning Club) in which they learned about safety, feelings and respect for themselves and others. In addition, the mothers and children also had the services of an advocate who helped them to access community resources.
Wagar and Rodway156 describe the programme as aiming at helping children to modify their responses to past experiences of witnessing violence, to develop problem-solving skills for future encounters, to address interpersonal responsibilities and attitudes regarding behaviours and foster self-esteem.
Psychoeducation for children who experienced sexual abuse
Trowell et al.155 describe psychoeducational group therapy for girls in which sessions were topic based, with information and suggestions given and explained in the group. The relationship between girls and the cotherapist leading the groups was also a focus, being linked with past and current relationships, losses and disruptions. In this intervention, carers also received group-based support.
Hébert and Tourigny163 evaluated a closed group led by two trained practitioners. The psychoeducational approach used combined a variety of therapeutic activities (e.g. group discussions, personal testimonies and stories, exercises and lectures). Some of the exercises targeted emotional regulation skills and cognitive coping strategies. Sessions included sex education and abuse prevention skills, and practice in social interactions with peers. Parents were invited to accompany the child for the first four sessions, which were used to promote positive child–caregiver interactions, improve communication and reinforce secure relationship. The intervention in Tourigny 2005166,167 and Tourigny 2007160 was also a closed group that used broadly the same approach, but without parental involvement. Each session used a similar format and was centred on a specific theme, such as disclosure of the abuse, the cycle of the abuse, consequences of abuse, relationship to the perpetrator, and so on.
Barth et al.161 provided psychoeducational groups for foster parents (both kin and non-kin), designed to provide an understanding of the types of behaviours presented by children who had been sexually abused and how these might best be managed.
The Children’s Treatment Program, evaluated by Duffany and Panos,162 comprised 12 lessons on important topics to the participating children and families, including My Body, Assertiveness, Touches, Who Can You Tell, Fears and Nightmares, and Inner Strength. The groups were open to avoid families having to wait, and siblings were also allowed to attend.
The study by Holland et al.164 evaluated a multimethod intervention for Aboriginal children in the Stól:lō Nation in British Columbia, Canada, who had been sexually abused. The intervention included group work that comprised psychoeducation and social skills training in a closed group format. No other information is provided other information.
The intervention in the study by Simoneau et al.159 was group work, with boys and girls organised by age (6–8 years, 9–13 years). They were accompanied by parents for the first five sessions. Children who missed more than two sessions, were offered entry into the next group. Focus of the groups was to (1) reduce sense of social isolation; (2) improve self-perception; (3) reduce behavioural difficulties; (4) improve the closeness with the caregiver; and (5) reduce or cease feelings of guilt linked to the abuse.
The intervention in the study by Santibáñez165 is also a multimethod programme that incorporates individual and group work-element focused, plus ‘unstructured daily life interventions’. Staff meet weekly to determine which interventions are to be used for the young people both in individual and group sessions, and daily life activities are prepared to encourage wider learning. Specifically, there are weekly activities with the young people tackling self-control and moral development. The self-control sessions are individual and each young person chooses an area to change (e.g. to reduce hitting, increase studying, participate more). Each young person is taught self-control step by step: contingency contract, learning how to self-control, choosing the problem to tackle, defining the behaviour to control or accept, behavioural self-observation, multimodal self-observation, self-evaluation and conclusions about the extent of the problem, questioning oneself about how to proceed, proposing realistic goals for change, learning at least one technique of self-control and how to apply it to another problem. For moral development, weekly group work sessions are held, at which there is discussion about questions that are of importance to each of the young people; friendship, characteristics of good friends, helping others, rules of the home, personal responsibility and losing control.
Number and duration of interventions
Duration of sessions ranged from 1 to 2 hours each, with sessions lasting 9–20 weeks (most are between 10 and 12 weeks), with the exception of Santibáñez,165 in which the programme lasted 4 months, and appears to have comprised weekly group and individual sessions plus everyday activities in the residential homes to promote generalisation of learning.
Outcomes: studies of psychoeducation
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Four measures of children’s post-traumatic stress symptoms were used across the seven153,155,160,163,166–168 studies assessing this outcome. Four153,160,166–168 studies used the Trauma Symptom Checklist (TSC). Overbeek et al.153 also used the Trauma Symptom Checklist for Young Children (TSCYC), Hébert and Tourigny163 used the Children’s Impact of Traumatic Events Scale-II (Wolfe, unpublished assessment instrument – available from VV Wolfe, Child and Adolescent Centre, London Health Sciences Centre, London, ON, Canada) and Trowell et al.155 used the 1989 version of Orvaschel’s PTSD scale406 – an extension of the KSADS instrument.268 The Trauma Symptom Checklist-40 (TSC-40408) was used by Wolfe et al.157 to assess symptoms of emotional distress.
Depression
Hébert and Tourigny163 assessed the impact of psychoeducation on childhood depression using the CDI.80 Trowell et al.155 used a shortened version of the KSADS.268
Self-harm
Self-harm was assessed in all three Tourigny studies160,166–168 using the Self-Injurious Behavior Questionnaire (Sadvosky, unpublished). Holland et al.164 also examined this outcome using administrative data.
Anxiety
Hébert and Tourigny et al.163 assessed anxiety as an outcome, using the RCMAS.256 This study also assessed symptoms of dissociation using the Child Dissociative Checklist.426
Behaviour problems
The effect of psychoeducation on children’s problem behaviour was examined by seven studies using various versions of the CBCL. The studies by Graham-Bermann et al.,151 Overbeek et al.,153 Hébert and Tourigny163 and Barth et al.161 used the Parent Report Form, and both studies by Tourigny et al.160,166–168 used the YSR Form.
Two studies assessed delinquency: Tourigny 2005166,167 used the Criminal and Delinquent Behaviours Questionnaire714 and Holland et al.164 used administrative data.
Antisocial and criminal behaviour was assessed using the Antisocial and Criminal Behavior Questionnaire715 in the study conducted by Santibáñez,165 who also investigated cognitive mediators of aggression using a 20-item Likert-type scale.427
Noether et al.158 used mother/carer ratings of the Behavioral and Emotional Rating Scale (BERS410) as the measure of the primary outcome measure.
The primary outcome in Duffany and Panos162 was recidivism (being re-abused or becoming abusers), assessed using the Youth Outcome Questionnaire.424
Self-control
Self-control was assessed in Santibáñez165 using the Shapiro Control Inventory428 in its Spanish version.
Social competence
In the Howell et al.152 study the primary outcome was enhancing social competence in children who had witnessed IPV which they measured using the Social Competence Scale (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group395).
Self-concept and self-adequacy
Sullivan et al.154 measured changes in the well-being of children who had been exposed to domestic violence using Harter’s Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC; 8- to 12-year-old version431). Hébert and Tourigny163 used the same measure as part of his larger battery of outcome measures for a group of sexually abused children.
All three Tourigny et al.160,166–168 studies assessed the impact of intervention on coping strategies using a French version of the Ways of Coping Questionnaire.419 They also deployed four of the five dimensions that make up the Empowerment Scale,421 namely optimism, self-efficacy, helplessness and justified anger plus the French version of the Children’s Attributions and Perceptions Scale.270
Sexual behaviour
Barth et al.161 and Holland et al.164 examined the impact of intervention on children’s sexual behaviours using the CSBI.423 Holland et al.164 also used administrative data.
Relationships
Healthy relationship skills were assessed in the study by Wolfe et al.157 using the Adolescent Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire.409
Two studies151,156 focused on children’s attitudes and beliefs about the acceptability of family violence. Graham-Bermann et al.151 assessed change in this outcome using the Attitudes About Family Violence scale,394 whereas Wagar and Rodway156 used a Child Witness to Violence Questionnaire (no reference provided) to assess children’s knowledge of wife abuse, who children feel are responsible, and their responses and attitudes to anger, their problem-solving abilities related to safety skills.
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of psychoeducation
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
It was not possible to judge whether sequence generation was adequately conducted, as five153–157 of the seven trials provided no other information other than participants were ‘randomly assigned’. These studies were therefore assessed as ‘unclear’ risk of bias for both sequence generation and allocation concealment.
Graham-Bermann et al.151 and Howell et al.152 both describe a modified, sequential random assignment procedure. In the study by Graham-Bermann et al.,151 the first seven children were assigned to the ‘child-only intervention’, the next seven children to the ‘child plus mother intervention’ and the next seven children to the wait-list control. In the Howell et al.152 study, the first five families were allocated to the experimental arm and the next five to the control arm, but the paper said nothing about allocation concealment. Both studies were assessed as ‘low risk of bias’ for sequence generation and unclear for allocation concealment.
Blinding of participants and personnel
Trowell et al.155 stated that it was not possible to blind participants. None of the other studies makes any reference to procedures to blind the participants or personnel, and neither is likely to have been undertaken, given the nature of the intervention. All studies were therefore assessed as ‘high risk of bias’ for this bias domain.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Outcome assessors were blinded in Overbeek et al.153 Graham Bermann et al.151 state that baseline interviews were conducted by researchers blind to group assignment and separate from those who provided the intervention; however, with the exception of child attitudes, the study151 relied solely on mother self-report and we judged this study151 overall to be of high risk of bias on this domain.
Wagar and Rodway156 state that group leaders conducted the pre-group interviews and administered the measurement tools, as well as facilitating the groups, and so this study was judged to be of high risk of bias for outcome assessment. Trowell et al.155 state that blinding of assessors was probably compromised because the children and their mothers often mentioned the specific therapy during assessment.
The absence of information on blinding of outcome assessors, together with the use of use of self-report and parent-reported measures in the studies by Howell et al.,152 Sullivan et al.154 and Wolfe et al.,157 suggest that outcome assessors were not masked in these studies, leading to a judgement of high risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
Three153–155 of the seven trials sought to minimise potential attrition bias by analysing participants based on ‘assigned treatment’ rather than ‘treatment completed’ and so were judged low risk of bias. Graham-Bermann et al.151 identify differential attrition as a cause of concern and this study was accordingly judged high risk of bias, as were the studies by Wagar and Rodway156 (which lost around 10% of children, all four from the experimental arm) and Wolfe et al.157 (which reported differential attrition of 21% experimental and 11% control and undertook completer-only analyses). Howell et al.152 was also deemed high risk of bias because 21 of the 113 children recruited to the study were missing from the final analyses (seven dropouts in the experimental group and 14 in the control group).
Selective outcome reporting
Although the studies all differed somewhat in their choice of outcomes, there appears to be no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results. In the absence of study protocols it is extremely difficult to assess the risk of selective outcome reporting. In that respect, almost all were assessed as unclear risk of bias. However, two156,157 of the included studies failed to report specific means and SDs for their post-intervention results and were therefore judged to be of high risk of bias.
Other sources of bias
Some other potential sources of bias were noted in two151,154 of the studies, including a suggestion that the sample was nationally unrepresentative151 and potential bias associated with paying participants for their continued involvement in the study.154
Full details of risk-of-bias assessments for each study can be found in Figures 33 and 34 in Appendix 10.
Quality assessment of quasi-experimental and controlled observational studies of psychoeducation
The quality of the three158–160 QEx studies and seven161–168 COSs was variable.
All studies158–168 provided a clear description of the study objectives, and, except for Holland et al.,164 all gave a clear description of the outcome and adequate descriptions of their participants.
Five159,162,164,166–168 of the included studies did not give an adequate description of the theoretical basis of their intervention. All of the studies158–168 at least partially addressed potential confounders in their research, and all provided a clear description of their findings; however, only two studies165,168 addressed potential adverse effects. Only two166–168 of the 10158–168 studies described the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up. Four158,160,162,163 of the 10 studies reported probability values for the main outcomes.
It was not possible to determine, for any of the studies, whether or not those who participated, and the staff involved in the study, were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited.
Blinding of participants or outcome assessors was not attempted in five161,162,164–167 of the studies, and, although it was unclear whether or not it was attempted in the remaining four158–160,163,168 studies, it seems unlikely that it would have been feasible because of the psychosocial nature of the intervention. Three of the studies164,166–168 did not recruit patients for intervention groups and controls from the same population.
Results: psychoeducation
Post-traumatic stress
Both Wolfe et al.157 and Overbeek et al.153 assessed the impact of the psychoeducational intervention on children’s post-traumatic stress, albeit with two measures (as measured by the TSC-40408 and the TSCYC,399 respectively). We were unable to combine these data in a meta-analysis. Based on ITT and completer analyses, Overbeek et al.153 found no differences between children in the experimental and control group; children in both groups improved.
Working with teenagers with histories of child maltreatment, Wolfe et al.157 reported improvements in symptoms of trauma.
Children’s PTSD was also assessed by Trowell et al.155 However, unlike the studies by Overbeek et al.153 and Wolfe et al.,157 this study155 compared two active treatments. In Trowell et al.,155 univariate analyses failed to identify a difference between those receiving group or IT on the PTSD dimension of ‘persistent symptoms of increased arousal’ used in the study.405 Following Cohen,716 the authors used an effect size of 0.5 as a threshold of moderate effect, and undertook no further analyses relating to PTSD symptoms.
Controlling for the impairment score on the KGAS405 in a multivariate analysis, the authors report a significant effect of IT for ‘the re-experiencing of trauma’ dimension of PTSD (baseline to 1- and 2-year follow-up, and baseline to exit), and – for the ‘persistence/avoidance of stimuli’ dimension – at baseline to first-year follow-up. When KGAS is replaced by the baseline score on the same dimensions, the significance of the effects is attenuated.
Children’s PTSD symptoms were also assessed in three COSs163,166–168 and one QEx study.160 Owing to the high risk of bias in all four of these studies,160,163,166–168 results were not incorporated into the meta-analyses and are instead presented narratively. Their results indicate that adolescents who were part of the psychoeducational intervention group improved significantly compared with controls on post-traumatic stress scores at both post test160,163,166,167 and 6-month follow-up.166,167 A fourth study168 found no statistically significant differences, although clinical measures suggested improvement for the treatment group compared with the control group.
Depression
In the trial conducted by Trowell et al.,155 the between-group differences on the KGAS failed to reach the threshold effect size adopted by the authors of 0.5 (following Cohen716). In the one CS163 that assessed childhood depression, participants in the intervention group demonstrated only marginally fewer symptoms of depression following the intervention.
Behavioural problems
Graham-Bermann et al.151 and Overbeek et al.153 assessed the impact of the psychoeducational intervention on child externalising and child internalising behaviours (as measured by the CBCL).
Externalising behaviours The pooled estimate using a random-effects model was –0.19 (SMD) (95% CI –0.45 to 0.06) (Figure 21). The I2-statistic indicates 0% of the variation in the point estimates is due to heterogeneity.
Internalising behaviours The pooled estimate using a random-effects model was –0.00 (SMD) (95% CI –0.25 to 0.25; p = 0.84) (Figure 22). The I2-statistic indicates 0% of the variation in the point estimates is due to heterogeneity.
Children’s behavioural problems were also assessed in three161,163,166–168 COSs and two158,160 QEx studies.
Four158,163,164,166,167 of the included studies found that post-test scores on behavioural measures were significantly lower for children who had received a psychoeducational intervention.
One study160 found that although intervention group participants (all girls) showed a significant decrease in internalising behaviours and social problems, change scores on externalising behaviours problems were not significant. One study168 found no statistically significant differences for internalising or externalising behaviours. Another study162 found that approximately one-third (15/47) of the children showed no change, or an insignificant worsening of behavioural and/or emotional symptoms following the intervention, and the last study161 reported that behaviour in both intervention and control groups worsened in equal measure from the pre-test to the follow-up.
Other outcomes
Self-injurious behaviours
Self-injurious behaviours were assessed in three164,166–168 COSs and one160 QEx study. Three160,166–168 of the studies demonstrated that children who were part of the intervention group improved significantly compared with control group participants. The fourth study164 indicated that there was no significant difference between groups for attempted/threatened suicide.
Anxiety and symptoms of dissociation
The same COS163 reported that participants in the intervention group demonstrated significantly less anxiety following the intervention and marginally fewer symptoms of dissociation.
Children’s sexual behaviours
Two161,164 COSs assessed children’s sexual behaviours. Holland et al.164 found no significant effects. Barth et al.161 reported a statistically significant treatment group effect (improvements) for two out of 36 items on the Coping Scales Inventory (CSI): ‘looks at people when nude’ and ‘shy about undressing’.
Delinquency
Three165–167 COSs assessed delinquency and antisocial behaviour. Tourigny 2005166,167 reported that children in the intervention group had a greater reduction in delinquent behaviours than children in the control group at the 6-month follow-up interview. Holland et al.164 and Santibáñez165 found no significant differences.
Self-control
The one165 study that assessed children’s self-control following the intervention found no significant difference between the intervention group and control group – despite finding some significant improvements – compared with control group participants – in levels of moral reasons and some of the factors believed to mediate aggression.
Coping strategies
Coping strategies were assessed in two166–168 COSs and one160 QEx study. All three160,166–168 studies demonstrated that children who were part of the intervention group improved significantly compared with control group participants in abuse-related attributions.
Self-competence
Children’s self-competence was assessed in one COS163 and one randomised CS.154 The RCT154 found that children who received the psychoeducational intervention demonstrated increased self-competence in their 4-month follow-up interview, whereas the self-competence of children in the control group remained relatively unchanged overall. Hébert and Tourigny163 found no significant difference in adjusted post-test scores for self-competence.
Sense of empowerment
Children’s sense of empowerment was assessed in two COSs166–168 and one QEx study.160 In all three studies,160,166–168 children who were part of the intervention group improved significantly compared with control group participants in abuse-related attributions.
Social competence
One RCT152 assessed participant’s prosocial skills, finding a significant improvement among children who received the intervention.
Relationships
In Wolfe et al.157 (RCT) intervention youths did not show the expected growth in healthy relationships skills over time.
Children’s attitudes and beliefs about the acceptability of family violence were measured in two randomised trials,151,156 using different measures (see Outcomes: studies of psychoeducation). Graham-Bermann et al.151 found that children who were part of the child and mother intervention made most improvement over time in attitudes about violence compared with those in the child-only and control groups. An ANCOVA of the pre-/post-treatment data by Wagar and Rodway.156 indicated significant differences in children’s ‘attitudes and responses to anger’ and ‘sense of responsibility for the parents & for the violence’. There were no significant differences between groups for ‘knowledge of safety & support skills’.
Effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions for maltreated children
Summary
We identified a large number of studies of psychoeducational interventions including seven151–157 randomised trials, three158–160 QEx studies and seven161–168 COSs. All included or comprised a group-based format, and although the groups used a variety of formats (activities, discussion, etc.) they generally included an educative component (focused on the nature of maltreatment suffered by the children), affect modulation, emotion regulation skills, coping and processing, social skills and addressing future safety.
Although the findings of this heterogeneous body of evidence vary somewhat, there is evidence of effectiveness in relation to symptoms of PTSD.
The evidence for improving children’s behaviour is more mixed. On the basis of ‘vote counting’ most report a positive impact on externalising, internalising, delinquency and antisocial behaviour and self-injurious behaviour. A minority report ‘no difference’ and Barth et al.161 report a worsening in children’s behaviour. Participants in the Barth et al.161 study were foster parents caring for sexually abused children, who rarely implemented the homework tasks required of them during this short intervention but who appreciated the intervention.
Where examined, similar results in favour of psychoeducation were reported for positive changes, such as improvements in coping, enhanced self-competence, social competence and sense of empowerment. Generally, the involvement of parents (often in parallel groups) was found to be helpful.
Completeness and applicability
All but four of the studies were conducted in North America. Two153,158 studies were conducted in the Netherlands, one165 study in Spain and one155 study in the UK.
Nine155,159–164,166–168 of the seventeen studies focused on sexually abused children, including the one UK study,155 which compared the effects of two manualised therapies: an individual psychotherapy and a group therapy that included psychoeducation. Six151–154,156,158 studies addressed the consequence of witnessing IPV, and two157,165 studies recruited children who had experienced other forms of maltreatment, including multiple abuse. The studies are broadly relevant to the UK, in terms of participants, settings and transferability of the interventions, but are sometimes limited by the cultural specificity of some interventions (e.g. Holland et al.164). There is clearly a bias towards evaluating interventions aimed at children who have been sexually abused or exposed to domestic violence, and less evidence about the usefulness of psychoeducational interventions to other groups of maltreated children.
Quality of the evidence
The seven151–157 trials of psychoeducation are generally of very poor quality, particularly in relation to detection bias and attrition bias (where only the study by Overbeek et al.153 was judged low risk of bias), and the quality of the quasi-experiments and COSs is variable. All in all, although the trends generally favour psychoeducation, there is a need for well-designed studies of psychoeducational interventions that are carefully designed in respect of their theories of change, carefully implemented, rigorously evaluated against agreed outcomes and outcome measures, and fully reported.
Economic evidence
One614 economic study, carried out in the UK, evaluated a group-based psychoeducation intervention for girls who had been sexually abused. The study614 used data from the Trowell et al.155 RCT, described above, and compared the psychoeducation intervention (n = 36) to individual psychotherapy (n = 35) for girls aged between 6 and 14 years.
Although described by the authors as a cost-effectiveness study,614 the economic evaluation should more accurately be classified as cost–consequences analyses, presenting costs and a range of disease-specific outcome measures separately. The study614 was carried out after the end of the Trowell et al.155 clinical trial, which precluded the prospective collection of resource-use data. As a result, the study614 was limited to a narrow economic perspective, including only the two interventions that were costed using nationally applicable unit costs and expressed in 1998–9 pounds sterling (£). No discounting was applied, despite a 2-year time horizon. Outcomes measured included psychiatric symptoms, global functioning, measures of PTSD and the experiences of carers.
Outcomes between the two groups were similar for the range of measures of effectiveness and costs were significantly higher for IT than the psychoeducation group therapy. The authors conclude that, with similar outcomes and higher costs, IT is less cost-effective than group therapy. However, they note that the logistics of setting up groups may mean children having to wait until there are sufficient numbers of a similar age before a group can start, resulting in a trade-off between potential savings to be gained from a group format and potential delays to treatment start for traumatised children. In addition, the study614 was limited in a number of important ways, including the narrow perspective, the lack of a TAU option or other control group, and the failure to formally combine costs and effects or explore uncertainty.
Group work with children
Five169–173 studies assessed the effectiveness of therapies that used the group format as an important therapeutic mechanism of change, but which are not described as psychoeducational.
Description of studies
Location of studies
Only one169 study took place in the UK. Of the COSs, two studies170,171 took place in Canada and two172,173 in the USA.
Sample sizes
All studies had small samples sizes. The COSs170,171 had sample sizes at baseline, ranging from a total of 12 participants171 to 70 participants.170 Monck et al.169 included 47 participants.
Participants
Age
Verleur et al.173 focused on teenagers with an age range of 13–17 years. The other studies focused on younger age groups, with ages ranging from 4 to 13 years.169–172
Maltreatment
All five169–173 studies recruited children and young people who had been sexually abused. De Luca et al.170 and McGain and McKinzey172 recruited only girls; Grayston and De Luca171 recruited only boys and Monck et al.169 recruited both boys and girls, although the sample included mainly girls (85%).
Interventions and comparisons
In the De Luca et al.170 study, participants in the intervention (sexually abused girls) were compared with girls with no known history of sexual abuse. Control group participants in the remaining studies received either TAU, or were placed on a wait-list control.
In four170–173 studies, the intervention was described as ‘group therapy’. Verleur et al.173 assessed the impact of group-based sexual education combined with group psychotherapy (no further information) led by same-sex (female) therapists. McGain and McKinzey172 set out the goals of the group-based programme, but do not detail the nature of the therapy/group process, other than to observe that it was ‘similar to hundreds of treatment programs provided to children who have been sexually abused’172 (p. 1168) and to differentiate it from other treatment programmes, such as those that are more behaviourally orientated, brief therapy programmes and crisis intervention programmes. The group intervention for boys in the study by Grayston and De Luca171 was run by therapists of both genders, supervised by a registered clinical psychologist. Sessions were said to follow ‘a consistent four-part format’ (described by De Luca and her associates717). Behaviour management techniques were used throughout to reduce disruptive behaviour and increase acceptable conduct. At the midpoint, the therapists also implemented a fixed-interval reinforcement schedule to further manage behaviour (described by Hack et al.535).
Monck et al.169 assessed the effectiveness of a family network treatment programme with or without group treatment.
Number and duration of sessions
Sessions in Grayston et al.171 and De Luca et al.170 were 90 minutes in duration, and were provided for 12 weeks171 and between 9 and 12 weeks.170
The group work programme evaluated by McGain and McKinzey172 continued weekly for 6 months, with the possibility for children to continue on to another cycle. Some children are reported to have spent between 9 months to 1 year in treatment.
The number of sessions ranged considerably, from 9–12 weekly sessions170,171 to 6–9 months of weekly sessions.172,173
No information was available on the structure of group work in Monck et al.169
Outcomes: group work with children
These studies169–173 had few outcomes in common, and, those that did, used different measures.
Anxiety
Anxiety was assessed in De Luca et al.,170 using the RCMAS/What I Think and Feel Questionnaire.293
Behaviour
Problem behaviours were assessed by three170–172 studies. De Luca et al.170 and Grayston and De Luca171 both used the CBCL,257 McGain and McKinzey172 used the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist438 and the ECBI.439
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of group work for children
A graphic summary of the risk of bias of the one group work trial is provided in Figures 35 and 36, Appendix 10.
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
Monck et al.169 stated that ‘allocation was made without the direct involvement of the researchers or the clinical team, and was achieved by blind choice of marked cards drawn from an envelope’169 (p. 12). Judgements of low risk of bias were made for both sequence generation and allocation concealment.
Blinding of participants, personnel and outcome assessors
This study169 did not make any reference to procedures to blind the participants or outcomes assessors. However, because of the nature of the intervention, it is unlikely to have been feasible to blind participants. The use of self-report and parent-reported measures also indicates that it was not possible to blind outcome assessors, suggesting this study169 had a high risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
This study169 analysed only the available data of 47 participants. However, there was insufficient information provided in the study169 regarding how many participants were originally assigned to each group. Therefore, this study169 was judged to be of unclear risk of bias.
Selective outcome reporting
There appears to be no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results, but, in the absence of prospective registration, we judged this aspect to be of ‘unclear’ risk of bias.
Other sorces of bias
No other identical sources of bias were identified in these studies.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Quality assessment of quasi-experimental and controlled observational studies of group work for children
The quality of the COSs was low. All five169–173 studies provided a clear description of the objectives and outcomes in their studies, with the exception of De Luca et al.,170 who did not clearly describe their outcomes. Only two171,172 studies provided clear descriptions of the characteristics of the patients included in the study,171,172 and only Grayston and De Luca171 provided a clear description of the intervention. Neither Grayston and De Luca171 nor De Luca et al.170 clearly described their main findings. No study adequately addressed potential adverse effects, nor did they report probability values for the main outcomes. Only one study172 provided information on recruitment of participants, the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up, and the representativeness of the staff (satisfactory). Determinations of the representativeness of others studies were not possible as a result of inadequate information. Blinding of participants and outcome assessors was not attempted in any of the studies.
Results: group work for children
No meta-analyses were possible for these interventions. There was only one randomised trial169 and a very heterogeneous group of COSs.
Depression
Monck et al.169 found no significant difference in depression between children participating in the family network treatment programme with group treatment and those participating in the family network treatment programme alone.
Anxiety
In the only study to assess anxiety, De Luca et al.170 found that, although anxiety scores for both intervention group (sexually abused) and the comparison group (no sexual abuse) decreased following the intervention, there was no significant difference between the two groups. The authors note that anxiety scores for both groups were low at baseline.
Self-esteem
The results of analyses in De Luca et al.170 indicated a statistically significant impact of group treatment for self-esteem for those girls who completed therapy. Monck et al.169 found no difference in measures of self-esteem following the intervention tested in the RCT.
Behavioural problems
Two170,172 of the three169,170,172 studies that examined the impact of group therapy on problem behaviour reported post-treatment improvement. Only in De Luca et al.170 were participants followed up for 1 year. In this study,170 parents reported sustained improvements in internalising and externalising behaviour problems at 9–12 months’ follow-up, although this reached statistical significance only for internalising behaviours. However, data were not collected on the behaviour of those in the comparison group.
Grayson and De Luca171 also found larger changes in pre–post treatment scores for problem behaviours among children in the intervention group, but the between-group differences were not statistically significant.
Sexual behaviour
Only Grayston and De Luca171 examined the impact of group therapy on sexual behaviour, finding that, although sexualised behaviour tended to decline following treatment, the changes – as measured by the Child Sexual Abuse Inventory – were not statistically different between treatment and control children. Verleur et al.173 reported that children in the experimental group improve significantly compared with the control group with regards to the measure of sexual awareness used in the study.
Self-esteem
One173 study assessed participants’ self-esteem following the intervention and found that, although the self-esteem for both intervention and control groups increased, there was a larger increase in the treatment group than the control group.
Effectiveness of group work interventions for maltreated children
Summary
We identified just five169–173 studies of group work (all with sexually abused children), of which only one169 study was a randomised trial. The trial169 found no evidence of the effectiveness of adding a group work component to a family network treatment in terms of reducing depression or improving children’s self-esteem. For the other studies, the limited information provided about the interventions, combined with heterogeneity in outcomes assessed and measures used, make it impossible to draw conclusions about the effectiveness of therapeutic group work for sexually abused children.
Completeness and applicability
The trial169 was conducted in the UK. The remaining four170–173 studies were conducted in North America. All focused on children who have been sexually abused. As indicated above, the studies provide little information that would allow these interventions to be replicated or to explore possible reasons for differences in reported effects.
Quality of the evidence
The trial conducted by Monck et al.169 ‘scored’ well in terms of risk of bias, and was one of the few studies that reported an adequate sequence generation and concealment of allocation. The COSs were judged to be overall of poor quality.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of group work with children who have been maltreated were located.
Psychotherapy/counselling
In this section, we review those studies that either stated that they were studies of psychotherapy or counselling per se, or that described an eclectic RBI. We deal with these together simply because it was not possible clearly to differentiate between the included studies in relation to the content of the therapies assessed.
Four155,174,178,179 studies assessed the effectiveness of psychotherapy without clearly describing their theoretical underpinnings. One178 study explored the value added of group psychotherapy when added to individual psychotherapy. One155 study is also described in the section on psychoeducation. The other two studies of interventions described as psychotherapy are those of Sullivan179 and Thun et al.174
We identified four175–177,180 studies that assessed the effectiveness of counselling interventions. Two assessed, respectively, the effectiveness of specific techniques for children in foster care, life story work175 and a mindfulness-based intervention.176 The other two studies are those of Cadol et al.177 and Downing et al.180
Description of studies
Study design
Four studies were randomised trials.155,174–176 Cadol et al.177 was a QEx study.
The Downing et al.,180 Sullivan179 and Nolan et al.178 studies were COSs.
Location
Nolan et al.178 was conducted in Ireland, and Trowell et al.155 in the UK. The other six studies174–177,179,180 were conducted in North America.
Sample sizes
Of the four trials, two were very small. Haight et al.175 was a feasibility study, with a small sample of just 23 randomised participants (15 completers). Thun et al.174 recruited 13 participants, but only 11 participants completed the intervention and follow-up assessments. Reddy et al.176 and Trowell et al.155 randomised 71 and 75 participants, respectively.
Cadol et al.177 recruited 140 participants and Sullivan179 recruited 72 participants. The remaining COSs were very small. Downing et al.180 had a sample size of 22 participants and Nolan et al.178 had a sample of 38 participants.
Participants
Age
Nolan et al.178 recruited children aged between 6 and 17 years old, and Trowell et al.155 recruited children aged 6–14 years. Thun et al.174 focused on teenage girls aged 16–18 years. Participants were aged 7–14 years in the study by Haight et al.,175 and 13–17 years in the Reddy et al.176 study. Downing et al.180 focused on children aged 6–12 years, and Cadol et al.177 focused on a slightly older population, with a mean age of 17.2 years. In the study by Sullivan179 participants were deaf children, aged 12–16 years, living in a residential school, who had been sexually abused by either dormitory staff or older pupils. This was the only CS179 that focused on disabled children.
Gender
The majority of children in the Nolan et al.178 study were female (92%). There were more males than females in Cadol et al.177 (69 males, 37 females). In the studies by Downing et al.,180 Haight et al.175 and Reddy et al.176 the samples were more evenly split, with girls being in the majority in Downing et al.180 and Reddy et al.176 and boys being in the majority in the Haight et al.175 study (nine, and six completers). All participants in the Trowell et al.155 study were girls.
Referral
Participants in the study by Nolan et al.178 were referred from eight urban and rural services for sexually abused children and adolescents. Those in the study by Trowell et al.155 were recruited from the authors’ own clinics and from professionals’ agencies in the community.
Haight et al.175 recruited children from Department of Children and Family Services caseworkers. Case managers approached eligible young people in foster care to invite them to participate in the study conducted by Reddy et al.176 Participants in Cadol et al.177 and Downing et al.180 studies were recruited from hospitals and private practitioners. Teenagers in the Thun et al.174 study were referred by the staff of the military-based programme to which they had signed up to assist them in getting their Graduate Equivalency Degree (having previously dropped out of school).
Maltreatment
Children in the Haight et al.175 study were in foster care and came from families in which they had experienced multiple forms of abuse, and whose parents misused methamphetamine. In Cadol et al.,177 the children had experienced both physical abuse and neglect. Downing et al.180 recruited children who had experienced sexual abuse. Reddy et al.176 provides no detailed information on maltreatment histories.
Participants in the studies by Trowell et al.,155 Thun et al.,174 Sullivan179 and Nolan et al.178 had all been subjected to sexual abuse.
Interventions and comparisons
Nolan et al.178 compared the effectiveness of IT with combined individual and group therapy (IGT). The purpose of these two interventions was to treat the ‘psychological sequelae of child sexual abuse’. Both interventions drew on the principles of a broad range of individual and group therapies (psychodynamic, client centred, CBT, etc.) and implementation varied between therapies and across clients. All therapists sought to provide their clients with a supportive therapeutic relationship that would enable them to process the psychological consequences of sexual abuse, and help them to develop the insights and skills needed to prevent further abuse. Those receiving group therapy also had the opportunity to realise that they were not alone in experiencing CSA, to enjoy peer support, and to benefit from peer-to-peer feedback on their experiences and their behaviour. On average, the duration of intervention ranged from 18 to 20 hours. This was the only intervention received by participants in the comparison group (those in the intervention group also received group psychotherapy).
Sullivan179 compared group psychotherapy developed at the Boys Town National Research Hospital with individual psychotherapy for survivors of abuse. After listing treatment goals, the reader is referred to an earlier paper for further information.718 Children in the Sullivan179 study met weekly with their therapist for 2 hours (because of the communication problems and need for signing) for 36 weeks. Control group participants received no treatment.
Thun et al.174 state that the group curriculum ‘followed a modified multidimensional model proposed by Lindon and Nourse (1994544) that incorporated a skills component, a psychotherapeutic component and an educative component’174 (p. 8) and, arguably, this study174 might, with additional information, have been included in the psychoeducational or group treatment grouping. In describing the intervention, Thun et al.174 emphasise the benefits of groups as a means of alleviating feelings of isolation and alienation, and fostering trust. Those in the comparison group had the option to avail of individual counselling, but none did.
The intervention used by Haight et al.175 was ‘Life Story Intervention’ (LSI), described by the study authors as a narrative and relationship-based mental health intervention. LSI was delivered in and around the children’s homes, on a one-to-one basis, by a range of professionals, including teachers, child welfare professionals and counsellors. The intervention was delivered over the course of a series of weekly 1-hour sessions for approximately 7 months. Control group participants were placed on a wait-list and received the intervention at the conclusion of the study.
Reddy et al.176 assessed the impact of Cognitively-Based Compassion Training (CBCT), described by the authors as a type of contemplative practice that is built on mindfulness practice and teaches active contemplation of loving kindness, empathy and compassion towards loved ones, strangers and enemies.719 It uses a variety of cognitive restructuring and asset-generating practices, with the long-term goal of developing the equanimity of mind that fosters acceptance and understanding of others. Participants were assigned to classes that met twice a week, for 1 hour, for 6 weeks. The control group was a wait-list control.
The interventions used in studies by Cadol et al.177 and Downing et al.180 were described as one-to-one counselling. No information was provided on the duration or number of counselling sessions, other than Downing et al.180 stating that sessions took place ‘near-weekly for approximately 1 year’. In Cadol et al.177 children in all arms received developmental testing, regular medical care and co-ordination of services, but only those in the experimental arm received the counselling. Participants in the control group in the Downing et al.180 study received reinforcement treatment (helping parents to focus on positive behaviour).
Trowell et al.155 compared brief, focused, individual psychoanalytic psychotherapy with group psychotherapy (comprising both psychotherapeutic and psychoeducational components).
Outcomes: psychotherapy/counselling
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Trowell et al.155 used the 1982 version of Orvaschel’s PTSD scale – an extension of the KSADS instruments.406 Reddy et al.176 used the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ448).
Trauma symptoms in Nolan et al.178 were assessed using the TSCC.260 Specific subscales used included anxiety, depression, anger, post-traumatic stress, dissociation, overt dissociation, fantasy dissociation, sexual concerns, sexual preoccupation and sexual distress.
Depression, anxiety, emotional disorder and conduct disorder
Trowell et al.155 used a shortened version of the KSADS.268 Reddy et al.176 used the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology–Self-Report442 to assess depression. Reddy et al.176 also assessed non-suicidal self- injurious behaviour using the Functional Assessment of Self-Mutilation (FASM443).
Nolan et al.178 assessed depression using the CDI.258 Specific subscales used included negative mood, interpersonal difficulties, ineffectiveness, anhedonia and negative self-esteem.
Child behaviour
Nolan et al.178 and Sullivan179 both assessed problem behaviours using the CBCL.294,418;720 Specific subscales used in Nolan et al.178 included total problems, externalising, internalising, withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behaviour and aggressive behaviour.
Cadol et al.177 assessed cognitive, physical, social, and emotional functioning of their participants using a range of measures, including the Bayley’s Scales of Infant Development,449 Bayley Infant Behavior Record451 and Child Behavioral Characteristics Questionnaire.
Downing et al.180 used parents’ and teachers’ behavioural observations to record sleep disturbance, sexual play with other children, enuresis, general misbehaviour and sexual self-stimulation.
Emotional self-regulation
Reddy et al.176 used the Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS446) to assess participants’ awareness and understanding of emotional experience, acceptance of emotions, ability to modulate emotional arousal and effective action in the presence of intense emotions.
Self-efficacy
Reddy et al.176 used the Children’s Hope Scale445 to assess agency and pathways (belief in one’s ability to develop successful call planning).
Self-esteem
Thun et al.174 assessed participants’ self-image using four subscales (see Results: psychotherapy/counselling) of the Offer Self-Image Questionnaire-Revised (OSIQ-R440), a personality test designed to measure self-image of adolescents aged 13–18 years.
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of psychotherapy/counselling
Figures 37 and 38, Appendix 10, present an overview of the risk of bias of RCTs of psychotherapy and counselling.
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
Thun et al.174 stated that participants were ‘randomly assigned’ but gave no other information and Trowell et al.155 simply said participants were randomised. Both studies were therefore assessed as ‘unclear’ for each domain.
Both Reddy et al.176 and Haight et al.175 were assessed as ‘unclear’ risk of bias for each of these domains. Although Reddy et al.176 state that the study used block randomisation to equalise numbers in each group, no information was provided that shed light on the sequence generation or allocation concealment.
Blinding of participants and personnel
Owing to the nature of the intervention, neither staff nor participants were likely to have been ‘blinded’ in the study by Thun et al.,174 and the study was therefore assessed as high risk of bias. In Trowell et al.,155 the authors state it was not possible to blind participants. Haight et al.175 stated that caregivers were not blind whether or not the child was receiving the intervention, and it would not have been possible to blind the children. Both studies were155,174 were therefore assessed as high risk of bias. The same judgement was made for Reddy et al.176
Blinding of outcome assessors
Assessors in Thun et al.174 were a faculty member and selected graduate students from a Marriage and Family Therapy programme at the University of Southern Mississippi but it is not clear whether or not they were blind to experimental conditions. This item was therefore assessed as unclear.
Reddy et al.176 provide no information on the blinding of outcome assessors. Haight et al.175 state that assessments were conducted by master’s level professionals who were not serving as the child’s community clinician, but it is not clear that they were unaware of the allocation status of the children. Both were therefore assessed as ‘unclear’.
Trowell et al.155 state that blinding of assessors was probably compromised because the children and their mother often mentioned the specific therapy during assesment, so was judged high risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
Thun et al.174 analysed only the available data, as two of the six participants randomised to the treatment arm dropped out. Haight et al.175 suffered significant attrition and analysed only the available data. Both studies174,175 were therefore judged as high risk of bias for incomplete outcome reporting.
No information is provided by Reddy et al.176 other than that caregivers in the wait-list control did not complete, post-treatment, the Inventory of Callous and Unemotional Traits-Parent Report (ICU-P447) – one out of six measures of children’s psychosocial functioning. This was therefore assessed as unclear risk of bias.
Trowell et al.155 was judged low risk of bias on these grounds.
Selective outcome reporting
There appears to be no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results from any of these four174–176 trials. However, in the absence of a published protocol or trial registration, we have judged all four to be of ‘unclear’ risk of bias.
Other sources of bias
No other potential sources of bias were identified in these studies.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Risk of bias: controlled observational studies of psychotherapy/counselling
Nolan et al.178 provided a clear description of the objectives, participants, intervention and outcomes in the study, and addressed potential confounders in their research. The study178 provided a good description of the characteristics of patients who were lost to follow-up. Statistical tests used were appropriate; however, actual probability values for the main outcomes were not reported.
Cadol et al.177 and Downing et al.180 provided a clear description of the objectives and participants in their studies, but only Downing et al.180 provided a clear description of the outcomes. Only Cadol et al.177 at least partially addressed potential confounders in their research. Although both of these studies177,180 provided a clear description of their findings, only Cadol et al.177 addressed the potential adverse effect of repeat abuse. Neither study described the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up; only Cadol et al.177 reported probability values for the main outcomes. It was not possible to determine whether or not the participants were asked to participate, or whether those that did, and the staff members involved in the study, were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited. Blinding of participants and of outcome assessors did not appear to be attempted by either study.
Sullivan179 failed to provide a clear description of the objectives, outcomes and participants in the study. The study179 did describe the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up, and reported actual probability values for the main outcome, and at least partially addressed potential confounders, but did not address potential adverse effects. It was not possible to determine whether or not participants had been asked to participate, or, whether they, or the staff involved in the study, were representative of the population from which they were recruited.
Results: psychotherapy/counselling
It was not possible to conduct a meta-analysis for any of the outcomes. We provide a short summary of the findings of each study, given their heterogeneity, their small samples, risk of bias and limited coverage of outcomes of interest.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
In Trowell et al.,155 univariate analyses failed to identify a difference between those receiving group and those receiving IT on the PTSD dimension of ‘persistent symptoms of increased arousal’ used in the study405 (data not provided, but the authors, following Cohen et al.,716 used an effect size of 0.5 as a threshold of moderate effect). No further analyses relating to this indicator of PTSD were undertaken.
Controlling for the impairment score on the KGAS405 in a multivariate analysis, the authors report a significant effect of IT for ‘the re-experiencing of trauma’ dimension of PTSD (baseline to 1- and 2-year follow up, and baseline to exit), and – for the ‘persistence/avoidance of stimuli’ dimension – at baseline to first-year follow-up. When KGAS is replaced by the baseline score on the same dimensions, the significance of the effects is attenuated.
For trauma symptoms, assessed by Nolan et al.178 using the TSCC,325 the only scores to improve were depression (p < 0.05) and anger (p < 0.01), and these improved for participants in both treatments, with no significant difference between the groups. No changes were detected for anxiety, post-traumatic symptoms, dissociation, overt dissociation, dissociation–fantasy, sexual concerns, sexual preoccupation and sexual distress.
Depression
In Nolan et al.,178 total depression score (p < 0.01), interpersonal problems (p < 0.05) and anhedonia (p < 0.01) all improved for both interventions, but there was no significant difference between groups. ‘Ineffectiveness’ was improved only in the combined IGT group (p < 0.01). Between-group difference in Trowell et al.155 failed to reach the threshold size of 0.5 adopted by the authors (KGAS).
Behavioural problems
Results in the study by Haight et al.175 indicate a significant group (experimental or control) by time (pre- or post-test) interaction on child externalising behaviour (p < 0.05), but no main effects. Results indicated that although experimental group externalising scores decreased, control group externalising increased over time. There were no other significant group or time effects for internalising behaviour scores or total problem scores.
Sullivan179 concluded that participants in the intervention group of this COS had significantly fewer behaviour problems than children not receiving individual psychotherapy. For boys, the treatment main effect was statistically significant for 10 of the 12 dependent variables assessed (total CBL; external and internal composite scales; nine CBL subscale scores). For girls, the main effect was significant for 5 of the 11 relevant variables (total CBL; external and internal composite scales; eight subscale scores). The numbers were very small, particularly for girls, and the authors point to a non-significant main effect for girls for the remaining variables.
Nolan et al.178 reported that total CBCL scores were reduced for both of the interventions (p < 0.01). In addition, internalising (p < 0.01) and externalising scores (p < 0.05) were also significantly improved by both interventions. CBCL subscales: withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed, social problems, attention problems and aggressive behaviour all improved with both interventions (p < 0.01). There was no significant difference between groups. Delinquent behaviour and thought problems remained unchanged. Results from the YSR indicated that there was no significant time or group effect on any YSR scales, indicating that there was no impact of therapy on any of the YSR scales.
Downing et al.180 reported that for the children in the counselling intervention group, parents reported a decrease in sleep disturbance, sexual play with other children, enuresis and general misbehaviour. However, there was no evident decrease in sexual self-stimulation.
Six weeks after the end of treatment, Reddy et al.176 found no differences on any measure of psychosocial functioning following CBCT, although the authors suggest that practice frequency was associated with increased hopefulness and trend for decrease in generalised anxiety.
Self-image
Thun et al.174 found no significant differences between the two groups on the four subscales of the OSIQ-R (Offer et al.440) used to measure self-image: impulse control, self-confidence, self-reliance and body image. Negative self-esteem remained unchanged in Nolan et al.178 (as assessed by the CDI).
For Cadol et al.177 results of comparisons of the three experimental groups and control indicate significant differences between the groups in the cognitive, physical, social and emotional areas. However, results also indicated that treatment techniques tested do not significantly affect the developmental performance of the participants.
Effectiveness of psychotherapy/counselling for maltreated children
Summary
A relatively small body of evidence was found pertaining to the effectiveness of psychotherapy/counselling interventions. Four155,174–176 studies were randomised trials, one177 study was a QEx study and three178–180 studies were COSs.
Five155,174,178,180,186 of the eight included studies recruited children who had been sexually abused. The remaining three studies recruited children who had experienced a variety of forms of maltreatment. The interventions were disparate, ranging from life story work with children in foster care, to cognitive-based compassion training – intervention built on mindfulness practice and cognitive restructuring.
Most studies compared psychotherapy with supportive counselling or no treatment, including wait-list controls. Nolan et al.178 compared the effectiveness of IT with combined IGT, and Trowell et al.155 compared individual psychotherapy with a group therapy that combined psychotherapuetic and psychoeducational components.
One study176 examined six outcome domains to assess the impact of compassion training, only one of which (depression) was examined in another study,178 which used a different measure. Four177–180 studies examined child behaviour as an outcome, but again all used different measures and had very different samples. It is therefore not possible, meaningfully, to draw any overall conclusions about the effectiveness of psychotherapy for maltreated children.
Completeness and applicability
All but Nolan et al.178 and Trowell et al.155 were conducted in the USA. Nolan et al.178 was conducted in Ireland and Trowell et al.155 in the UK. Most of the interventions described have residents with therapies available in the UK, for example one-to-one counselling,177,180 the therapies provided in Nolan et al.178 for sexually abused girls and the LSI described by Haight et al.175 Given the dominance of counselling and current recommendations to increase access to counselling for troubled children, the dearth of evidence to support the effectiveness of psychotherapy or counselling for maltreated children is of concern.
Quality of the evidence
Four174,175,178,180 of the studies, including two174,175 of the three trials, were extremely small. The trials were poorly reported, with low risk of judgements made only in relation to selective outcome reporting (and this is probably generous). The overall quality of the non-randomised studies was judged to be poor.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of psychotherapy/counselling for children who have been maltreated were located.
Peer mentoring
We identified two181,182 studies that assessed the effectiveness of peer-mentoring interventions.
Description of studies
Sample sizes
Both181,182 studies had baseline sample sizes of just 36 and 46 participants, respectively.
Participants
Maltreatment
Children in both studies had experienced both physical abuse and neglect.
Interventions and comparisons
The intervention used in both studies181,182 was resilient peer treatment (RPT), designed to promote the development of social competencies of preschool children in the context of classroom play mediated by a resourceful peer.
In Fantuzzo 1988181 the control group pairs met in the same setting as the treatment group pairs for the same number of play sessions under identical conditions, except that their peer was instructed to respond positively to social initiations but to refrain from initiating social interactions. In Fantuzzo 1996,182 control group participants were paired with a classmate of average interactive play ability.
Number and duration of sessions
In both181,182 studies, sessions lasted 15 minutes each, and were spread out over a 2-month period, with no more than three sessions occurring per week.
Outcomes
Interactive peer play was assessed by Fantuzzo 1996182 using an observational coding system.
Social skills were assessed in the Fantuzzo 1996182 study, using the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS).379
Problem behaviours were also assessed in Fantuzzo 1996182 using the SSRS.379
Positive social behaviours were assessed by Fantuzzo 1988181 through the use of the same observational coding system, the SSRS.379
Psychological adjustment was assessed by Fantuzzo 1988181 using the Preschool Behavior Questionnaire.455
Pre-academic progress was assessed in Fantuzzo et al.181 using the Brigance Diagnostic Inventory of Early Development.456
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trials of peer-mentoring interventions
The risk of bias across the two181,182 included trials of peer mentoring is summarised in Figures 39 and 40 in Appendix 10.
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
Both trials181,182 stated their participants were ‘randomly assigned’, but gave no other information on the method of random allocation and allocation concealment. Both were judged ‘unclear’ risk of bias for each of these domains.
Blinding of participants and personnel
One181 study made no reference to procedures to blind participants or personnel, but the other study182 stated that ‘teachers, play supporters and data collectors were not informed of the maltreatment status of the participants throughout the study’182 (p. 1284). Fantuzzo et al.182 was therefore judged to be of low risk of bias. The other181 study was assessed as being of unclear risk of bias.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Both181,182 studies stated that children were observed by trained raters who were blind to both the assignment of children to conditions and the specific hypotheses of the study, resulting in judgements of low risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
Both181,182 studies were assessed being of low risk of bias, as both were able to minimise potential attrition bias by analysing participants based on ‘assigned treatment’ rather than ‘treatment completed’.
Selective outcome reporting
Both181,182 studies differed in their choice of outcomes, and there appears to be no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results. However, in the absence of a published protocol or trial registration, we categorised these studies181,182 as ‘unclear’ risk of bias.
Other sources of bias
No other potential sources of bias were identified in these studies.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Results: peer-mentoring interventions
It was not possible to conduct a meta-analysis for any of the outcomes.
Interactive peer play
Children assigned to the RPT intervention showed significantly higher levels of interactive play and significantly less solitary play post test than the children in the control condition. No significant group differences were found for the social attention and non-play categories by Fantuzzo 1996.182
Social skills
Children in the RPT group scored significantly higher than those in the control group on the Self-Control subscale and the Interpersonal Skills subscale. However, no significant group differences were found on the Verbal Assertion subscale (Fantuzzo 1996182).
In Fantuzzo 1988,181 results indicated that children in the peer-mentoring group demonstrated significantly increased levels of positive social interaction, whereas levels of social interaction remained the same for the control group.
Problem behaviours
Children who received the RPT intervention displayed significantly lower levels of both internalising and externalising behaviour problems (Fantuzzo 1996182).
In Fantuzzo 1988,181 results indicated that, post test, the children in the peer group demonstrated either a similar, or a decreased, level of problematic behaviours, whereas control groups seemed to demonstrate an increase in problematic behaviour.
Preacademic progress
Results indicated that there was no significant group effect for pre-academic progress (Fantuzzo 1988181).
Effectiveness of peer mentoring for maltreated children
Summary
The two studies of peer mentoring identified in our search were both randomised trials, were both conducted in the USA by the same researcher. The results provide evidence of a range of benefits of peer mentoring for maltreated children, although both studies were small and the follow-up periods were short.
Completeness and applicability
Evidently this evidence base is limited, but it represents the only rigorous evaluation of therapeutic day care programmes aimed at addressing the needs of neglected of physically abused children. This could be of relevance to the UK, where there is an increasing move to intervene with children in the school setting.
Quality of the evidence
Compared with most of the included trials, the quality of these peer-mentoring studies is moderately good.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of peer-mentoring for children who have been maltreated were located.
Intensive service models
Covered in this section are a number of interventions designed to help children in substitute care or children in specialist day-care settings. They fall into three broad categories:
- co-ordinated care.198
In this context, treatment foster care is used to describe a number of intensive interventions targeted at children in foster care, rather than – as is sometimes done – as the name of an intervention. Because of the heterogeneity of these studies,145,183–198 we discuss each group of studies separately, beginning with treatment foster care.
Description of studies
Study design
Four of the controlled studies were randomised trials.145,183–191 Fisher et al.192 and Graham et al.193 were COSs. Graham et al.193 used a subsample recruited from the trial conducted by Fisher et al.183–188 Biehal et al.145 embedded a small randomised trial within a QEx case–control study.
Location
Five studies183–193 were conducted in the USA, whereas the study by Biehal et al.145,146 was conducted in the UK.
Sample sizes
Three trials183–191 had moderate baseline sample sizes of 100, 117 and 156 participants, respectively. Sample sizes in the COSs were small, with just 30 participants in the Fisher et al.192 study and 37 participants in the Graham et al.193 study. Biehal et al.145,146 randomised just 34 young people; a further 185 were included in the QEx sample.145
Participants
Age
For the treatment foster care studies, the mean ages of participants in the RCTs were 5.94 years;183–188 11.54 years;189 and 10.46 years.190,191 In the study by Biehal et al.,145,146 the young people were aged 10–17 years (although the intended recruitment age range was 11–16 years).
The mean age of participants across the COSs was 2.35 years in the Fisher et al.192 study and 6.11 years in the Graham et al.193 study. In Taiissig et al.190,191 the age range was 9–11 years.
Maltreatment
All studies145,183–193 focused on children who had experienced a combination of physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse and neglect.
Interventions and comparisons
Control group participants included those who had received RFC in four183–189,192,193 studies. In the studies by Biehal et al.145,146 and Taussig et al.190,191 they received services as usual.
Four145,146,183–188,193 studies were of MTFC. MTFC has been described as a community-based, multimodal ‘wraparound’ intervention for children and young people with challenging behaviour. It makes use of a ‘team approach’, by which foster parents (and, where applicable, biological parents/future carers) are trained to provide a therapeutic home environment for children. Foster parents received intensive preservice training and post placement they received support and supervision by means of daily telephone contacts, weekly home visits by foster parent consultant, a weekly support group and 24-hour on-call crisis intervention. Children received services from a behaviour specialist. When appropriate, the family therapist worked with the biological family to teach the same parenting skills used by programme foster parents in order to promote generalisation of treatment gains and facilitate reunification.
Three183–188,192,193 of the four MTFC studies used the Multidimensional Treatment Foster Care Program for Preschoolers (MTFC-P).
Smith et al.189 evaluated the effectiveness of a manualised intervention targeting the prevention of behaviour problems for girls in foster care at the point of transition to middle school. In the summer prior to middle school entry, both foster parents and the girls they were caring for participated in separate six-session, group-based interventions, followed by ongoing training and support to foster carers and girls throughout the first year of middle school. The girls groups focused on ‘setting personal goals; establishing and maintaining positive relationships with peers and adults; effective decision-making and problem-solving strategies; developing support systems for reaching goals; and modelling, practising, and reinforcing adaptive behaviours’ (p. 271). Foster parents groups were focused on establishing and maintaining stability in the home, preparing the girls for school and preventing early adjustment problems during the transition. They were taught how to use a behavioural reinforcement system modelled after systems used in MTFC.
The intervention evaluated in the Taussig et al.190 trial was called Fostering Healthy Futures (FHF), which the authors describe as a preventative mental health intervention, consisting of two components, specifically a manualised skills group and one-on-one mentoring.
Number and duration of sessions
In the Biehal et al.145,146 study, the duration of an MTFC placement was intended to be around 9 months, prior to a young person’s return to his or her birth family or to an alternative placement. With one exception, young people in the study145,146 spent between 5 and 11 months in MTFC placements.
In the Taussig et al.190,191 study the skills groups met for approximately 30 weeks for 1.5-hour weekly sessions. The mentoring component of Taussig et al.’s190,191 FHF programme provided 30 weeks of one-on-one mentoring for each child. Mentors spent 2–4 hours of individual time each week with their mentees.
In the Fisher et al.183–188 study, children are said usually to receive services for between 6 and 9 months. Information on number and duration of sessions is unavailable from Fisher et al.192 (other than that reported above). No information on ‘dose’ was provided by Graham et al.193
In the Smith et al.189 study, the summer groups comprised six sessions across 3 weeks, followed by weekly 2-hour meetings (foster parent meeting; one-on-one session for girls) throughout the first year of middle school.
Outcomes: treatment foster care
Post-traumatic stress disorder
No study assessed the impact of the intervention on PTSD, but the primary outcome in Taussig et al.190,191 was mental health functioning, measured using two scales from the TSCC,325 specifically the post-traumatic stress scale, and the dissociation scales (see Child behaviour, below).
A multi-informant index of mental health problems was also derived in this study, based on TSCC325 scores, the internalising scales of the CBCL269 and the Teacher Report Form (TRF269).
Salivary cortisol
Children’s salivary cortisol levels were used to assess hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activity in three183–188,192,193 of the four included studies.
Child behaviour
Smith et al.189 assessed children’s internalising problems, externalising problems and prosocial behaviour using the Parent Daily Report Checklist.458 Taussig et al.190,191 used the Internalising scales of the CBCL269 – Youth Report and TRFs. Data were combined with TSCC325 scores to create a mental health index (see Results: treatment foster care).
As one of two primary outcomes in the RCT, Biehal et al.145,146 used the standardised Health of the Nation Outcome Scales for Children and Adolescents (HoNOSCA382) as a measure of emotional and behavioural difficulties. It was selected because it enabled the synthesis of large quantities of data gathered from multiple informants and sources including a range of standardised measure (and which could accommodate variable completeness of information).
Biehal et al.145,146 also used a range of standardised measures, including the CBCL,257 SDQ721 and DAWBA-AD.
One of the COSs192 assessed child behaviours problems using the Early Childhood Inventory (ECI462).
Attachment-related behaviour
Children’s attachment-related behaviour towards foster parents was assessed by Fisher et al.183–188 using the PAD.221
Biehal et al.145,146 used the Development and Well-Being Assessment-Attachment Disorder (Minnis et al., unpublished manuscript), consisting of 26 items about behaviours associated with the International Classification of Diseases-Tenth Edition diagnoses of attachment disorder.
Children’s social functioning
Participants’ coping skills were measured in Taussig et al.190,191 using the Positive and Negative Coping scales of the CSI.459 In this study,190,191 participants’ perceived self-competence was also measured using the social acceptance and global self-worth scales of the SPPC.413,431
Biehal et al.145,146 used the CGAS292 to assess children’s general adaptive functioning.
Permanency
Placement outcomes were assessed by three145,146,183–188,190,191 studies. Taussig et al.190,191 recorded the number of placement changes over the 18-month study period of the study, whether or not a child had experienced a new placement in a residential treatment centre (RTC) during that time or had attained permanency by 1-year post intervention, plus the types of permanence attained (i.e. adoption or reunification with family). Fisher et al.183–188 recorded the type of permanent placement, and success or failure of a subsequent permanent placement. Biehal et al.145,146 recorded care placement type.
Quality of life
Taussig et al.190,191 assessed children’s quality of life using the Life Satisfaction Survey.402 Related to quality of life, the authors also recorded children’s recent and current use of mental health services and psychotropic medications (based on caregiver and self-report), and their levels of social support, using scores from The People in My Life-Short Form.460
Other
Biehal et al.145,146 also gathered data relating to children’s engagement in education and training, including type of provision received.
Cognitive control
Cognitive control and response monitoring was assessed by Fisher et al.183–188 using a computerised flanker task, which includes red and green circles as stimuli and trial-by-trial performance feedback.722 We include a brief description, taken from one of the papers, as this outcome measure is not commonplace. A small fixation point was displayed in the centre of a computer screen.
For each trial, a warning cue is presented for 200 ms before a horizontal row of five 1-in. circles, with the central circle directly above the fixation point, is shown for 700 ms. The task comprises congruent trials, which consist of five red circles or five green circles, and incongruent trials, which consist of a central red circle flanked by green circles or a central green circle flanked by red circles. A 30:70 ratio (congruent trials–incongruent trials) is used. Participants are required to respond within 1100 ms. Performance feedback, which consists of a 1-in. face, is then presented for 1050 ms; a smiling face indicates a correct response and a frowning face indicates an incorrect response. The intertrial interval varies in length from 0 to 500 ms. The 20-min task consists of three blocks of 60 trials each. In the current study, the STIM stimulus presentation system (James Long Company, Caroga Lake, NY) was used to control the task presentation and to record the behavioral and electrophysiological measures for each trial. The children sat approximately 24 in. from a 14 in. computer monitor and held a button box with a red pushbutton and a green pushbutton. Prior to beginning the task, color vision, color familiarity, and comprehension of task terminology were assessed. The children were instructed to press the button that corresponded with the color of the central circle regardless of the color of the flanking circles. They were told to respond quickly and correctly. The children completed eight practice trials to ensure task comprehension.
Reproduced with permission from Bruce et al.,183 p. 5
Risk of bias: randomised trials of Treatment Foster Care models
Sequence generation
Smith et al.189 stated that a coin flip was used, and for the randomised part of the Biehal et al.145,146 study, the authors state that the randomisation sequence was generated by a computer-generated random numbers. Both were therefore assessed as ‘low risk of bias’. Fisher et al.183–188 provide no information on sequence generation, and Taussig et al.190,191 stated that all children were manually randomised, by cohort (five), but provided no further information. Both183–189 studies were therefore judged unclear risk of bias.
Allocation concealment
No study provided information on allocation concealment, and all were judged unclear risk of bias.
Blinding of participants and personnel
No study made any reference to procedures to blind the participants or personnel, and judgements of high risk of bias were allocated to each.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Smith et al.189 and Fisher et al.183–188 state that research staff and interviewers were blind to participants’ group assignment. Biehal et al.145,146 describe the study as single blind, with outcome assessors blind to which arm data belonged (although the assessors were not those who provided/gathered the information). Taussig et al.190,191 attempted to mask independent interviewers to condition but records that some children did spontaneously disclose treatment condition which could not be controlled for. All studies were judged low risk of bias.
Incomplete outcome data
Fisher et al.183–188 was able to minimise potential attrition bias by making use of a full information maximum likelihood estimator in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA) which allows for the inclusion of participants with partial data on dependent variables, and was judged to be of low risk of bias. Smith et al.189 provided insufficient information regarding dropouts and missing data and was judged unclear risk of bias. Taussig et al.190,191 undertook data analyses using ITT principles and the study was judged to be of low risk of bias. Biehal et al.145,146 state that data from the RCT sample was analysed on ITT principles (subject to the availability of outcome data), with imputation for missing data. Although this study145,146 suffered from differential attrition (12 of the 34 young people randomised to MTFC did not receive it) the data sources meant that only one person was lost to follow-up. Overall, we judged this study to be of low risk of bias for incomplete outcome data.
Selective outcome reporting
Two trials were registered: Taussig et al.190,191 (NCT00809315) and Fisher et al.183–188 (NCT00701194). Both were judged to be of low risk of bias, as the authors report on all outcomes listed in the trial registration (although specific measures were not identified). In the absence of a published protocol, the studies by Biehal et al.,145,146 and Smith et al.189 were judged to be ‘unclear’.
Other sources of bias
A potential additional bias was noted in Fisher et al.,183–188 in which randomisation occurred prior to recruitment into the study, and performance was not assessed prior to the intervention. Therefore, differences between the two groups may have existed prior to the intervention. No other potential sources of bias were identified in this studies.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Quality assessment of controlled observational studies of intensive service models
The quality of the two192,193 COSs was very similar. Both provided a clear description of the objectives of the study, the outcomes, the participants and the interventions. Both addressed potential confounders in their research, and both provided a clear description of their findings. Both studies reported actual probability values for the main outcomes.
However, neither study addressed potential adverse effects, nor described the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up.
It was not possible to determine whether or not participants were asked to participate, or whether or not those who did participate, and the staff members involved in the study were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited for any of the studies. Statistical tests used to assess the main outcomes appeared appropriate. Blinding of participants or outcome assessors was not attempted and it seems unlikely it would have been feasible owing to the psychosocial nature of the intervention.
Results: treatment foster care
There were no common outcomes recorded across any of the included RCTs. Therefore, it was not possible to conduct a meta-analysis for any of the outcomes.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Post-traumatic stress disorder symptomatology was assessed only in the Taussig et al.190,191 study, which reported no significant effect post intervention or 6 months’ follow-up. The study190,191 results indicate that intervention participants had significantly fewer dissociation symptoms than control group participants, as measured by the TSCC at the 6-month follow-up (p = 0.02).
Mental health
Taussig et al.190,191 reported that intervention participants had significantly fewer mental health problems than control group participants at the 6-month follow-up (p = 0.003), as measured by the multi-informant index of mental health problems. There was no significant difference between intervention and control participants for recent or current use of psychotropic medications (post test or 6-month follow-up) but there was a significantly lower rate of recent mental health therapy usage at the 6-month follow-up for intervention participants (p = 0.04). Intervention participants also had significantly better quality of life at the end of the intervention (p = 0.006) but this difference failed to reach significance at 6 months (p = 0.38).
Children’s behaviour problems
Smith et al.189 (RCT) indicated that the participants who were part of the MTFC intervention demonstrated significantly lower internalising problems (p < 0.01) and lower externalising problems (p < 0.01) at 6 months post baseline than the control participants. However, this study189 found that intervention participants did not have higher prosocial behaviour than the control participants (p > 0.05).
Fisher et al.192 (RCT) found that participants who were part of the MTFC intervention demonstrated significantly improved behavioural adjustment scores (p < 0.05), as measured by the ECI.
Taussig et al.190,191 reported no significant difference between intervention and control groups for Positive and Negative Coping skills, perceived self-competence, or social support at post test or 6-month follow-up.
In the study by Biehal et al.,145,146 MTFC participants had slightly better outcomes at follow-up than UC, as measured by HoNOSCA (adjusted MD –1.04, 95% CI –6.21 to 4.13) but this was not statistically significant (p = 0.68).
Children’s attachment-related behaviour
Children’s attachment-related behaviour towards foster parents was assessed by Fisher et al.,183–188 and indicated that children in the intervention condition made significant increases in secure attachment behaviour (p < 0.05) and significant decreases in avoidant behaviour (p < 0.05) compared with children in the RFC condition. However, treatment was not associated with changes in resistant behaviour. No between-group differences were found in the Biehal et al.145,146 study.
Salivary cortisol
Children’s salivary cortisol levels were taken by three of the included studies.183–188,192,193 Although the results of these studies indicated some improvement over time, including daytime patterns, which showed the salivary cortisol levels of the intervention group becoming closer to the community comparison group of non-maltreated children,192 and a distinct pattern of change in cortisol slope across the days,189 none of these results reached statistical significance. Moreover, Fisher et al.183–188 indicated that control group participants showed significantly greater morning-to-evening cortisol level decreases following placement changes than the experimental group (p < 0.05), a negative outcome.
Permanent placement outcomes
Permanent placement outcomes were assessed by the studies of Fisher et al.183–188 and Taussig et al.190,191 In Fisher et al.,183–188 participants in both groups entered permanent placements at approximately equal rates, but the number of placement failures between the two groups was significantly different, with 10% (n = 3) failing in the intervention group compared with 36% (n = 9) in RFC [χ2(1) = 5.11; p = 0.02]. One child in the RFC group experienced two placement failures (no child had two placement failures in the early intervention fostering group).
In the study by Taussig et al.,190,191 the results indicated a significantly lower rate of placement changes for the intervention group than the control group (p = 0.04), and a significantly lower rate of new placements in a RTC (p = 0.03). There was also a higher rate of permanency attained for intervention group participants than for the control group participants (p = 0.004).
For children whose parents retained parental rights, significantly more intervention youth had reunified at 1 year post intervention than the control youth (p < 0.05). Taussig et al.190,191 also report that 26% of intervention children had been adopted 1 year post intervention, compared with 8% of control children, but numbers were too small for conventional statistical tests.
Children’s social functioning
In the study by Biehal et al.,145,146 MTFC participants had slightly better outcomes at follow-up than UC participants (adjusted MD 1.3, 95% CI –7.1 to 9.7) but this was not statistically significant (p = 0.75). Biehal et al.145,146 also found no differences for school engagement or school exclusion.
No between-group differences were found in Taussig et al.190,191 in relation to the Coping Inventory or the Self-Perception Profile.
Cognitive control and response monitoring
Cognitive control and response monitoring was assessed by Fisher et al.183–188 using a computerised flanker task. There were no group differences on the behavioural measures of cognitive control (p > 0.05) or response monitoring (p > 0.05). A significant group effect was indicated observed on the electrophysiological measures of response monitoring. Children who received the intervention were significantly more responsive to performance feedback than foster children who received services as usual (p < 0.05).
Economic evidence
Two economic evaluations of intensive service models of care for children who have been maltreated were located in the systematic review (Wood et al.616 and Lynch et al.615), both carried out in the USA.
Wood et al.616 was a cohort study comparing the costs and outcomes for families receiving a child abuse prevention service (n = 26) and families receiving UC (n = 24). The intervention was home-based counselling/psychology and the therapists were available 24 hours a day, 7 days a week, for a period of 4–6 weeks, providing practical help, FT and liaison with schools other community services in order to reduce the risk of out-of-home placement.
The evaluation, most accurately described as a cost–consequences analysis, had a 1-year follow-up and took a limited economic perspective, focusing on the service provider and including the cost of the interventions and any out-of-home placements. Outcomes were measured in terms of family functioning and whether or not the children stayed at home. Methods of costing were not outlined.
Children in the families that received the intensive intervention were significantly more likely to remain at home than families receiving UC and costs were significantly lower, as a result of the lower use of placements. However, this study616 was carried out some years ago and the methods are severely limited, with no random allocation, small sample sizes, failure to report the results for all measures of outcome, lack of incremental analysis and no assessment of uncertainty.
Lynch et al.615 evaluated the net benefit of MTFC-P entering new foster placements. The study615 used data from the Fisher et al.186 RCT (see subsection Intensive service models, Description of studies) and compared MTFC-P (n = 57) with RFC (n = 60) for children aged between 3 and 5 years.
The evaluation was conducted from a public agency perspective, including health, social care and education. Resources used were valued using nationally applicable published unit costs and were reported in 2008 US dollars (US$). Outcomes were assessed in terms of the primary measure for the clinical trial – the rate of placement permanency for each group, where permanent placement included reuniting with the biological parent, adoption by a relative or non-relative adoption. Costs and outcomes were assessed over a 24-month period and did not appear to be discounted.
Permanent placement rates were higher for the MTFC-P group than the RFC group, although the difference was not significant. Average total costs were significantly lower for MTFC-P and the incremental average net benefit was positive for all levels of willingness to pay for improvements in outcome, indicating that the value of the benefits of MTFC-P was greater than the costs. The authors conclude that MTFC-P is highly likely to have a positive net benefit for increasing permanent placements in comparison with RFC. Although no agreed level of willingness to pay for increases in placement permanence exists, the authors note that lack of permanent placement is associated with a variety of negative outcomes for young people, and suggest that willingness to pay for reductions in neglect and abuse is high. The study615 suffers from relatively small sample sizes and the lack of a generic measure of outcome associated with an agreed level of willingness to pay.
Therapeutic residential and day care services
No controlled studies that assessed the effectiveness of therapeutic residential care interventions were identified. We found three controlled studies194–197 that assessed the effectiveness of Therapeutic Day Programme interventions, of which only one was a randomised trial.194 The others195–197 were COSs.
Description of studies
Sample sizes
The randomised trial had a sample size of 61 participants at baseline and 35 participants assessed at follow-up. The COSs195–197 had sample sizes of 34 and 70 participants, respectively.
Location
All three therapeutic day programme studies were conducted in the USA.
Participants
Age
At recruitment, the mean age of participants in the RCT was 11 months (SD 7 months) for the intervention group and 13 months (SD 8 months) for the control group. Participants were followed up to a mean age of 12 years (SD 7 months) for the intervention group and 13 years (SD 7 months) for the control group.194 The mean age of participants across the COSs195–197 ranged from 3 years195,196 to 4.8 years.197
Maltreatment
All three194–197 studies recruited children who had experienced a combination of physical abuse and neglect.
Interventions and comparisons
Two195–197 of the interventions were identified by the study authors as ‘therapeutic day treatment program’. The third194 was identified as the Childhaven therapeutic child care program. All three194–197 studies aimed to improve outcomes for children who had experienced maltreatment.
Control group participants in the included studies received either CPS as usual,194 no intervention,195,196 or were part of a wait-list control group.197
Number and duration of sessions/treatment
Limited information was provided by the RCT study194 regarding the duration and number of treatment sessions.
The therapeutic day-treatment programme195–197 was described as a classroom-based therapy lasting 6 hours per day, 5 days per week, for approximately 8–9 months.
Outcomes
Behaviour problems
Moore et al.194 used all three report forms (caregivers, teachers and youth report) of the CBCL269,294,418 to assess the impact of intervention on children’s behaviour.
Moore et al.194 also measured drug and alcohol use with the Winters Personal Experience Screening Questionnaire723: participants’ involvement in the legal system, using juvenile court files, and – using school files – their involvement in special classes, their grades, special help, disciplinary actions, suspensions, or expulsions.
Risk of bias: randomised trial
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
In the absence of information about sequence generation or allocation concealment, a judgement of unclear risk of bias was made in relation to Moore et al.194
Blinding of participants and personnel
No information was provided on the blinding of participants or personnel, and the study194 was judged as being of high risk, as neither is likely to have been feasible.
Blinding of outcome assessors
Interviewers were initially blind to group assignment, but caregivers did have the opportunity to mention their treatment experiences, which is likely to have indicated group assignment. A judgement of high risk of bias was therefore made.
Incomplete outcome data
There is a high risk of bias related to incomplete outcomes data, as this study194 analysed only available case data.
Selective outcome reporting
There is no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results, but, in the absence of trial registration or a published protocol, we judged Moore et al.194 as being ‘unclear’. See Appendix 10.
Quality assessment of controlled observational studies
The quality of the two COSs was variable. Both provided a clear description of the objectives of the study, the outcomes, the participants, the interventions and the findings. Only Culp 1991197 addressed potential confounders in the research. Neither study addressed potential adverse effects, nor did they describe the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up. Only Culp 1991197 reported actual probability values for the main outcomes. It was not possible to determine whether the subjects asked to participate, the subjects who did participate, and the staff members involved in the study were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited for any of the studies. Statistical tests used to assess the main outcomes appeared appropriate. Blinding of participants or outcome assessors was not attempted and it seems unlikely it would have been feasible owing to the psychosocial nature of the intervention.
Results: therapeutic day care
It was not possible to conduct a meta-analysis for any of the outcomes, as only one relevant randomised trial was found.
Children’s problem behaviour
Moore et al.194 found that for the CBCL (YSR), more children in the control group than in the intervention group scored in the clinical range, but this difference reached significance only for the aggressive behaviour subscale (p < 0.05). However, teachers perceived a higher rate of problems for children in the intervention group, although these differences failed to reach significance.
This study194 found that a significantly higher percentage of control youths showed drug and alcohol use (p < 0.05). Furthermore, control group youths were first arrested at a significantly younger age than intervention youths (p < 0.01). There was no significant difference between groups for delinquency records or delinquency episodes and no significant difference for property crime. However, control youths were arrested significantly more often for serious/violent crimes than intervention group youths (p < 0.05).
This study194 found no significant difference between groups for special classes, grades and special help. Although there was a higher rate of disciplinary actions, suspensions or expulsions for control group participants, this difference reached significance only for disciplinary actions for fighting (p < 0.05).
Children’s developmental progress
Culp 1987195,196 measured children’s developments progress on all five subscales of the EIDP, and found that children who were enrolled in the day-treatment programme scored significantly higher than control children on perception/fine motor skills (p < 0.001), cognition (p < 0.001), gross motor skills (p < 0.01), social/emotional (p < 0.001) and language development (p < 0.05). Culp 1991197 found similar results for perception/fine motor (p = 0.05), cognition (p = 0.01) and social/emotional (p = 0.01). However, this study197 found no significant difference for gross motor skills (p = 0.07).
Children’s self-concept
Children’s self-concept was measured by Moore et al.194 (RCT) and Culp 1991197 (COS). Culp 1991197 found children receiving the intervention scored significantly better in cognitive competence (p = 0.05), physical competence (p = 0.01) and maternal acceptance (p = 0.02). However, there was no significant group effect for peer acceptance (p = 0.59). Likewise, in the Moore et al.194 study, results indicate that intervention youths rated themselves higher than control youths on the Social Acceptance Scale [F(1,32) = 3.8; p < 0.07].
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of therapeutic residential or day-care services for children who have been maltreated were located.
Co-ordinated care
We identified just one example of co-ordinated care evaluated with a CS.198
Description of studies
Study design and location
The study was a randomised trial198 that took place in the USA.
Sample size
This study198 recruited 45 families with 72 children.
Participants
Children ranged from birth to 17 years old, with a relatively even split of males and females (47% male, 53% female). Participants were recruited from the Department of Social Services.
Maltreatment
Children had experienced multiple forms of abuse, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse and neglect.
Intervention and comparisons
This interagency intervention established a partnership between one private provider and three state agencies that served maltreated children who were compulsorily removed to out-of-home care, named the ‘Charleston Collaborative Project’, which aimed to:
- reduce risk factors to promote child safety and child functioning and caregiving functioning, thereby allowing return of children to their families in a timely fashion
- provide cost savings by delivering effective and focused interventions at the time children enter care to reduce both the number of children requiring more intensive and costly services and the length of time children remain in state custody
- improve service system efficiency by co-ordinating care.
These goals were to be achieved by creating a single point of entry and a ‘seamless system for providing services’.198 The providers of Charleston Collaborative Project included an assessment worker, a therapist and a service co-ordinator. Control group participants in this study received TAU.
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trial of co-ordinated care
Sequence generation
Low: participants were randomly assigned using a table of random numbers.
Allocation concealment
Unclear: inadequate information provided.
Blinding of participants and personnel
High: the study198 did not discuss this issue, but blinding these parties is not possible in such an intervention.
Blinding of outcome assessors
High: some data were collected from the child’s caseworker, and the study198 does not indicate that the researcher who interviewed family members or caseworkers was blind to allocation status of families.
Incomplete outcome data
Low: only available data were analysed but attrition was low (42/45 families were retained at last data collection point, and no family dropped out of treatment).
Selective outcome reporting
Unclear: there appears to be no evidence that other outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results. However, the study198 did not provide post intervention means for any of the outcomes, only latent growth curve data.
Other sources of bias
Unclear: where multiple children in one family were abused, one child was randomly selected for inclusion in the analyses of child functioning. No further details were provided.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Results: co-ordinated care
Although this study198 found total behaviour problems (as measured by the CBCL) to decrease significantly for both intervention and control groups, there was no significant difference between the two groups for any of the measures post test.
The effectiveness of intensive service models
Summary
There is growing evidence for the effectiveness of MTFC for children who have experienced one or more of physical, emotional, sexual abuse and neglect. The evidence suggests that MTFC can exert a positive influence on children’s internalising and externalising problems, and may be able to improve children’s emotional self-regulation (as measured by saliva cortisol), but more evidence is required. The study by Fisher et al.183–188 suggests that MTSC may increase a child’s chances of a successful permanent placement for children in out-of-home care. MTSC children in this study198 also made significant increases in secure attachment behaviour towards foster carers and significant decreases in avoidant behaviour when compared with children in RFC.
Although included within the group of TFC studies, the intervention evaluated by Taussig et al.190,191 is more accurately described as enhanced foster care, in contrast with the MTFC examined in the other three studies. Overall, the findings by Taussig et al.190,191 provide limited support for the effectiveness of the mentoring and skills group intervention evaluated in improving children’s mental health. Taussig et al.190,191 reports a lower rate of placement changes and lower rates of new placements in RTCs for children who participated in the mentoring and skills group.
The results of Moore’s evaluation194 of the Childhaven Therapeutic Early Intervention suggest some improvement in children’s self-concept, in reduced drug and alcohol use and age at first arrest, and in self-reported levels of aggression. However, the evidence is limited, and no differences were found for other measures (e.g. school performance) to support its effectiveness. In contrast, the two studies by Culp et al.195–197 suggest that therapeutic day care of the kind provided can improve children’s development across a variety of domains and also (perhaps as a consequence) improve their self-concept.
There is no evidence for the effectiveness of the model of co-ordinated care assessed by Swenson et al.198
Completeness and applicability
All183–196 but one145 of the studies were conducted in the USA. The UK study145 was a replication of MTFC. This study,145 conducted in difficult circumstances, failed to reproduce the results reported from the USA studies, but this may be due to design and implementation features (not necessarily the responsibility of the research team) and the quality of the comparison group: MTFC was compared with routine services in the UK, compared with limited provision of custodial care in the USA. The evidence from Fisher et al.,183–188 although limited, suggests that intensive fostering support such as that provided in MTFC might well be able to improve the success rates for permanent placements following foster care, although this also needs to be tested in locations in which the profile of provision is closer to that of the UK than the USA.
Quality of the evidence
Overall, the quality of the evidence from trials is moderate.
Economic evidence
One economic evaluation,198 carried out in the USA, explored the costs and outcomes of a co-ordinated model of care, the Charleston Collaborative Project, for maltreated children. The paper is also reported in the effectiveness section above, as it is a combined effectiveness and economic paper. The study used a RCT design to compare the collaborative model of care (n = 48) to UC (n = 24) for maltreated children aged between 1 and 16 years.
The paper presents costs and outcomes separately, so would best be described as a cost–consequences analysis. Participants were followed up post treatment and 3 months post treatment. Effects included caregiver and child psychosocial functioning. Costs focused on the interventions under consideration, out-of-home placements and health services. Costs are reported in US dollars but the financial year applied was not reported.
Despite the collaborative project being more expensive than UC, total costs were lower as a result of lower placement and other service-use costs. Statistical analyses were not reported for all cost categories or total costs, so it is unclear if this difference is significant. The authors note that much of the difference in out-of-home placement costs was due to two young people in the UC group, which, given very small sample sizes (total n = 39 for cost data), may be the result of chance. The authors caution against drawing any firm conclusions from the cost data. Outcomes were similar across the two groups and as no formal economic evaluation, combining costs and outcomes, was undertaken no clear conclusion can be drawn.
Activity-based therapies
We identified six studies199–204 that assessed the effectiveness of interventions that engaged the child in activity-based interventions, grouped as follows:
- animal therapy.204
Proponents of these therapies would possibly emphasise the differences between them, as opposed to the similarities. For this reason, we provide a ‘grouped but differentiated’ account of the descriptive elements of the studies (designs, sample sizes, etc.) and separate accounts of the findings of the included studies.
Description of studies
Study design
Play therapy
One of the three studies of play therapy was a randomised trial202 and one a quasi-experimental study.203 The third was a COS.201
Animal therapy
The only study of animal therapy204 was a COS.
Location
Arts therapy
Pretorius and Pfeifer200 took place in South Africa and Brillantes-Evangelista199 was conducted in the Philippines.
Play therapy
The Udwin203 study (QEx) was conducted in the UK. The other two201,202 studies took place in the USA.
Animal therapy
The Dietz et al.204 study took place in the USA.
Sample sizes
Arts therapy
Pretorius and Pfeifer200 and Brillantes-Evangelista199 recruited samples of just 25 and 33 participants, respectively.
Play therapy
Both the RCT203 and QEx study202 were very small, with samples of just 34203 and 38.202 The COS201 had a somewhat larger sample of 88 participants.
Animal therapy
Dietz et al.204 recruited 153 participants.
Participants
Arts therapy
Pretorius and Pfeifer200 recruited girls aged 8–11 years. Brillantes-Evangelista199 focused on a slightly older population of both girls and boys (64% female), aged 13–18 years. Participants in both199,200 studies were recruited from either children’s homes200 or shelters for abused children.199
Play therapy
The QEx study203 focused on younger children, aged 3–6 years. The remaining studies201,202 focused, respectively, on populations with age ranges of 8–17 years202 and 12–21 years.201 Although the COS201 focused solely on girls, the remaining two202,203 studies focused on a fairly equal numbers of males and females.
Participants in the studies by McDonald and Howe202 and D’Andrea et al.201 were recruited from residential facilities. No information was provided by Udwin203 on how participants were recruited.
Animal therapy
Dietz et al.204 focused on children and adolescents aged 7–17 years, the majority of whom (94%) were female. Participants were recruited from CPS, law enforcement, the county district attorney’s office, and the local children’s hospital.
Maltreatment
Arts therapy
One study200 focused on girls who had been sexually abused, whereas the other study199 focused on children who had experienced both physical and sexual abuse.
Play therapy
There was limited information regarding the specific types of maltreatment across all three play therapy studies.201–203 The children were identified as having been ‘abused’,202 as having experienced ‘parental neglect and/or abuse’,203 or a mixture of ‘physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect’.201
Animal therapy
The intervention in Dietz et al.204 was directed at children who had experienced sexual abuse.
Intervention and comparison
Arts therapy
This intervention used in Pretorius and Pfeifer200 was a structured group art therapy programme, aimed at reducing depression, anxiety, sexual trauma and low self-esteem. Brillantes-Evangelista199 had two experimental groups: (1) a visual arts group and (2) a poetry group. Both studies included a ‘no treatment’ control group.
Play therapy
The intervention used in the RCT202 was a challenge/initiative programme that aimed to enhance self-concept using co-operative and adventure games. The QEx study203 assessed the effects of imaginative play training. The COS201 assessed the impact of a sports-based intervention called ‘Do the Good’ (DtG), which was designed using trauma-informed treatment principles. Control group participants across all three play therapy studies201–203 received some form of active comparison, including playing other games with the same researcher but with no debriefing,202 engaging in a variety of activities unrelated to make-believe203 and structured activities part of participant’s TAU routine.201
Animal therapy
Dietz et al.204 used canine animal-assisted therapy (AAT). Two forms of treatment were compared with a ‘no treatment control’. The treatments were an ‘AAT with therapeutic stories’ group and an ‘AAT without therapeutic stories’ group.
Frequency and duration of sessions
Arts therapy
Both199,200 art therapy studies aimed at alleviating depression and PTSD and provided treatment over the course of eight weekly sessions.
Play therapy
All three play therapy interventions used a group-based approach, the duration of which ranged from 30 minutes203 to 60 minutes.201,202
Animal therapy
Both treatments were provided in 12 weekly sessions.
Outcomes: activity-based interventions
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Depression
Depression was assessed in Pretorius and Pfeifer200 using both the TSCC473 and human figure drawing (HFD474). It was also assessed in Brillantes-Evangelista199 using the Self-Rating Depression Scale (as cited in Coroner and Fischer472).
Behaviour
Self-concept
Children’s imaginary play
Play therapy
Udwin203 assessed dimensions of children’s imaginative play by recording observations of (1) imagination, (2) positive affect, (3) concentration, (4) aggression, (5) peer interaction, (6) adult interaction, (7) peer co-operation, and (8) adult co-operation. Udwin et al.203 also assessed participant’s fantasy predisposition, using Guilford’s Unusual Uses Test477 and a storytelling task using Children’s Apperception Test.478
Risk of bias: randomised controlled trial of play therapy
Sequence generation and allocation concealment
Unclear risk: McDonald and Howe202 simply stated that participants were ‘randomly assigned’ but gave no other information on the method of random allocation or allocation concealment.
Blinding of participants, personnel and outcome assessors
High risk: McDonald and Howe202 made no reference to procedures to blind the participants or personnel (unlikely to have been feasible) or to outcomes assessors. The study202 was judged high risk of bias on each domain (participants and personnel, and outcome assessors).
Incomplete outcome data
Low risk: there is nothing to suggest that outcome data were incomplete for this study.
Selective outcome reporting
Unclear risk: there is no evidence that outcomes were planned and then omitted from the results, but we have only the paper to rely on, and it is an older paper.
Other sources of bias
The same researcher was the ‘recreation leader’ for both groups, leading to a judgement of high risk of bias.
Summary details of the risk-of-bias assessments of these trials can be found in Appendix 10.
Quality assessment of quasi-experimental and controlled observational studies of activity-based interventions
Arts therapy
The quality of the two199,200 QEx studies was very similar. Both studies provided a clear description of the objectives, intervention and outcomes in the study, although neither study adequately described their participants. Neither study addressed potential confounders in their research. Although both studies provided a clear description of their findings, neither addressed potential adverse effects. Both studies failed to describe the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up. Statistical tests used were appropriate and both studies reported actual probability values for the main outcomes. It was not possible to determine whether or not the subjects asked to participate, or whether or not those who did participate, or the staff members involved in the study, were representative of the population from which they were recruited. Blinding of participants and of outcome assessors did not appear to have been attempted by either study.
Play therapy
The quality of the QEx study203 and COS201 was variable. Both studies provided a clear description of the objectives, participants and outcomes in the study. Both studies only partially addressed potential confounders in their research. Although both studies provided a clear description of their findings, neither study addressed potential adverse effects. Both studies failed to describe the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up, and only one study203 reported actual probability values for the main outcomes. It was not possible to determine whether the subjects asked to participate, the subjects who did participate, and the staff members involved in the study, were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited for any of the studies. Blinding of participants and of outcome assessors did not appear to have been attempted by either study.
Animal therapy
The quality of Dietz et al.204 was variable. The study provided a clear description of the objectives, participants, intervention and outcomes, and addressed potential confounders. But, despite providing a clear description of findings, the study failed to address potential adverse effects. The study also failed to describe the characteristics of patients lost to follow-up. Statistical tests used were appropriate and actual probability values for the main outcomes were reported. It was not possible to determine whether the subjects asked to participate, the subjects who did participate, and the staff members involved in the study were representative of the entire population from which they were recruited for any of the studies. Blinding of participants and of outcome assessors did not appear to have been attempted.
Results: activity-based interventions
It was not possible to conduct a meta-analysis for any of the outcomes, as only one RCT was identified.
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Arts therapy
In the study by Pretorius and Pfeifer,200 no statistically significant difference was found between the intervention and control group for trauma as measured by the TSCC or the HFD. In the Brillantes-Evangelista199 study, trauma scores decreased for both intervention groups and increased for the control group, but this change reached statistical significance only for the visual arts group (p = 0.011).
Animal therapy
The scores for the children in the animal therapy (dogs) with stories group decreased significantly more than the animal therapy (dogs) without stories group for all of the subscales of the TSCC, except sexual concerns (p < 0.001). This included subscales of anxiety, depression, anger, PTSD and dissociation. In addition, the animal therapy without stories group decreased significantly more than the no animal therapy (dogs) group (p < 0.001).
In the sexual concerns group, children in the animal therapy (dogs) with stories group had scores that decreased significantly more than those in either the no animal therapy or animal therapy (dogs) without stories groups (p < 0.001).
Depression
Arts therapy
Although the intervention group participants demonstrated a greater improvement in depression across both199,200 studies, this difference reached statistical significance only in the Pretorius and Pfeifer200 study (p = 0.001) when measured by the HFD. The scores on the TSCC failed to reach statistical significance in Pretorius and Pfeifer.200
In the study by Brillantes-Evangelista199 the improvement in scores reached statistical significance only for the poetry intervention group (p = 0.0445). Improvement in scores for the visual arts intervention group and the control group failed to reach statistical significance.
Anxiety
Arts therapy
Pretorius and Pfeifer200 found no statistically significant difference between groups for anxiety as measured by the TSCC, although the experimental group evidenced statistically significantly lower post-test scores than the control group on anxiety (p = 0.000) as measured by the HFD.
Effectiveness of activity-based interventions
Summary
We were able to identify only two199,200 studies of art therapy, two201,203 studies of play therapy and one204 study of an animal therapy. One202 of these was a randomised trial, three199,200,203 were QEx and two203,204 were COSs. The outcomes targeted in these studies were heterogeneous, as were the interventions, making it extremely difficult to draw conclusions about their effectiveness. The small samples in these studies and the generally poor quality exacerbate this problem.
Completeness and applicability
Given the popularity of activity-based therapies in the UK and elsewhere, it is striking that we were able to identify so few studies. Even more striking is the absence of rigorous studies of music therapy, which appears to be extremely popular in the treatment of traumatised children, particularly young children.
Economic evidence
No economic evaluations of activity-based therapies for children who have been maltreated were located.
- Cognitive–behavioural therapy
- Cognitive–behavioural therapy: children who have been physically abused
- Cognitive–behavioural therapy: children who have experienced different types of maltreatment
- Economic analysis: cognitive–behavioural therapy
- Relationship-based interventions
- Systemic interventions
- Psychoeducation
- Group work with children
- Psychotherapy/counselling
- Peer mentoring
- Intensive service models
- Therapeutic residential and day care services
- Co-ordinated care
- Activity-based therapies
- Results - The effectiveness, acceptability and cost-effectiveness of psychosocia...Results - The effectiveness, acceptability and cost-effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for maltreated children and adolescents: an evidence synthesis
- Cost analysis - Aquatic therapy for children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy: a...Cost analysis - Aquatic therapy for children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy: a pilot feasibility randomised controlled trial and mixed-methods process evaluation
- Summary of findings and assessment of feasibility - An intervention to improve t...Summary of findings and assessment of feasibility - An intervention to improve the quality of life in children of parents with serious mental illness: the Young SMILES feasibility RCT
- Results - A high-dose preparation of lactobacilli and bifidobacteria in the prev...Results - A high-dose preparation of lactobacilli and bifidobacteria in the prevention of antibiotic-associated and Clostridium difficile diarrhoea in older people admitted to hospital: a multicentre, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel arm trial (PLACIDE)
- Discussion - OPTIMA prelim: a randomised feasibility study of personalised care ...Discussion - OPTIMA prelim: a randomised feasibility study of personalised care in the treatment of women with early breast cancer
Your browsing activity is empty.
Activity recording is turned off.
See more...